Somatosensory system

The somatosensory system is a part of the sensory nervous system. The somatosensory system is a complex system of sensory neurons and pathways that responds to changes at the surface or inside the body. The axons (as afferent nerve fibers), of sensory neurons connect with, or respond to, various receptor cells. These sensory receptor cells are activated by different stimuli such as heat and pain, giving a functional name to the responding sensory neuron, such as a thermoreceptor which carries information about temperature changes. Other types include mechanoreceptors, chemoreceptors, and nociceptors and they send signals along a sensory nerve to the spinal cord where they may be processed by other sensory neurons and then relayed to the brain for further processing. Sensory receptors are found all over the body including the skin, epithelial tissues, muscles, bones and joints, internal organs, and the cardiovascular system.

Touch is a crucial means of receiving information. This photo shows tactile markings identifying stairs for visually impaired people.
Somatosensory cortex in purple (motor cortex in green). From Blausen Medical.[1] Lateral view is shown. See below for coronal cross-sections, shown in black and white images, from Gray's Anatomy.

Somatic senses are sometimes referred to as somesthetic senses,[2] with the understanding that somesthesis includes the sense of touch, proprioception (sense of position and movement), and (depending on usage) haptic perception.[3]

The mapping of the body surfaces in the brain is called somatotopy. In the cortex, it is also referred to as the cortical homunculus. This brain-surface ("cortical") map is not immutable, however. Dramatic shifts can occur in response to stroke or injury.

System overview

This diagram linearly (unless otherwise mentioned) tracks the projections of all known structures that allow for touch to their relevant endpoints in the human brain.

Mechanical

The four mechanoreceptors in the skin each respond to different stimuli for short or long periods.

Merkel cell nerve endings are found in the basal epidermis and hair follicles; they react to low vibrations (5–15 Hz) and deep static touch such as shapes and edges. Due to a small receptive field (extremely detailed info) they are used in areas like fingertips the most; they are not covered (shelled) and thus respond to pressures over long periods.

Tactile corpuscles react to moderate vibration (10–50 Hz) and light touch. They are located in the dermal papillae; due to their reactivity they are primarily located in fingertips and lips. They respond in quick action potentials, unlike Merkel. They are responsible for the ability to read Braille and feel gentle stimuli.

Lamellar corpuscles determine gross touch and distinguish rough and soft substances. They react in quick action potentials, especially to vibrations around 250 Hz (even up to centimeters away). They are the most sensitive to vibrations, and have large receptor fields. Pacinian reacts only to sudden stimuli so pressures like clothes that are always compressing their shape are quickly ignored.

Bulbous corpuscles react slowly and respond to sustained skin stretch. They are responsible for the feeling of object slippage and play a major role in the kinesthetic sense and control of finger position and movement. Merkel and bulbous cells (slow-response) are myelinated; the rest (fast-response) are not. All of these receptors are activated upon pressures that squish their shape causing an action potential.[4][5][6][7][8][9][10]

Neural

Gray's Anatomy, figure 759: the sensory tract, showing the pathway (blue) up the spinal cord, through the somatosensory thalamus, to S1 (Brodmann areas 3, 1, and 2), S2, and BA7
Gray's Anatomy, figure 717: detail showing path adjacent to the insular cortex (marked insula in this figure), adjacent to S1, S2, and BA7

All afferent touch/vibration info ascends the spinal cord via the posterior (dorsal) column-medial lemniscus pathway via gracilis (T7 and below) or cuneatus (T6 and above).

Cuneatus sends signals to the cochlear nucleus indirectly via spinal grey matter, this info is used in determining if a perceived sound is just villi noise/irritation. All fibers cross (left becomes right) in the medulla.

The postcentral gyrus includes the primary somatosensory cortex (Brodmann areas 3, 2 and 1) collectively referred to as S1.

BA3 receives the densest projections from the thalamus. BA3a is involved with the sense of relative position of neighboring body parts and amount of effort being used during movement. BA3b is responsible for distributing somato info, it projects texture info to BA1 and shape + size info to BA2.

Region S2 (secondary somatosensory cortex) divides into Area S2 and parietal ventral area. Area S2 is involved with specific touch perception and is thus integrally linked with the amygdala and hippocampus to encode and reinforce memories.

Parietal ventral area is the somatosensory relay to the premotor cortex and somatosensory memory hub, BA5.

BA5 is the topographically organized somato memory field and association area.

BA1 processes texture info while BA2 processes size + shape info.

Area S2 processes light touch, pain, visceral sensation, and tactile attention.

S1 processes the remaining info (crude touch, pain, temperature).[11][12][13][14]

BA7 integrates visual and proprioceptive info to locate objects in space.[15][16]

The insular cortex plays a role in the sense of bodily-ownership, bodily self-awareness, and perception. Insula also plays a role in conveying info about sensual touch, pain, temperature, itch, and local oxygen status. Insula is a highly connected relay and thus is involved in numerous functions.

Structure

The somatosensory system is spread through all major parts of the vertebrate body. It consists both of sensory receptors and afferent neurons in the periphery (skin, muscle and organs for example), to deeper neurons within the central nervous system.

General somatosensory pathway

A somatosensory pathway will typically have three long neurons:[17] primary, secondary, and tertiary (or first, second, and third).

  1. The first neuron always has its cell body in the dorsal root ganglion of the spinal nerve (if sensation is in parts of the head or neck not covered by the cervical nerves, it will be the trigeminal nerve ganglia or the ganglia of other sensory cranial nerves).
  2. The second neuron has its cell body either in the spinal cord or in the brainstem. This neuron's ascending axons will cross (decussate) to the opposite side either in the spinal cord or in the brainstem.
  3. In the case of touch and certain types of pain, the third neuron has its cell body in the VPN of the thalamus and ends in the postcentral gyrus of the parietal lobe.
Touch can result in many different physiological reactions. Here, a baby laughs at being tickled by an older sister.

Photoreceptors, similar to those found in the retina of the eye, detect potentially damaging ultraviolet radiation (ultraviolet A specifically), inducing increased production of melanin by melanocytes.[18] Thus tanning potentially offers the skin rapid protection from DNA damage and sunburn caused by ultraviolet radiation (DNA damage caused by ultraviolet B). However, whether this offers protection is debatable, because the amount of melanin released by this process is modest in comparison to the amounts released in response to DNA damage caused by ultraviolet B radiation.[18]

Tactile feedback

The tactile feedback from proprioception is derived from the proprioceptors in the skin, muscles, and joints.[19]

Balance

The receptor for the sense of balance resides in the vestibular system in the ear (for the three-dimensional orientation of the head, and by inference, the rest of the body). Balance is also mediated by the kinesthetic reflex fed by proprioception (which senses the relative location of the rest of the body to the head).[20] In addition, proprioception estimates the location of objects which are sensed by the visual system (which provides confirmation of the place of those objects relative to the body), as input to the mechanical reflexes of the body.

Fine touch and crude touch

The cortical homunculus, a map of somatosensory areas of the brain, was devised by Wilder Penfield.

Fine touch (or discriminative touch) is a sensory modality that allows a subject to sense and localize touch. The form of touch where localization is not possible is known as crude touch. The posterior column–medial lemniscus pathway is the pathway responsible for the sending of fine touch information to the cerebral cortex of the brain.

Crude touch (or non-discriminative touch) is a sensory modality that allows the subject to sense that something has touched them, without being able to localize where they were touched (contrasting "fine touch"). Its fibres are carried in the spinothalamic tract, unlike the fine touch, which is carried in the dorsal column. [21] As fine touch normally works in parallel to crude touch, a person will be able to localize touch until fibres carrying fine touch (Posterior column–medial lemniscus pathway) have been disrupted. Then the subject will feel the touch, but be unable to identify where they were touched.

Neural processing of social touch

The somatosensory cortex encodes incoming sensory information from receptors all over the body. Affective touch is a type of sensory information that elicits an emotional reaction and is usually social in nature, such as a physical human touch. This type of information is actually coded differently than other sensory information. Intensity of affective touch is still encoded in the primary somatosensory cortex, but the feeling of pleasantness associated with affective touch activates the anterior cingulate cortex more than the primary somatosensory cortex. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data shows that increased blood oxygen level contrast (BOLD) signal in the anterior cingulate cortex as well as the prefrontal cortex is highly correlated with pleasantness scores of an affective touch. Inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the primary somatosensory cortex inhibits the perception of affective touch intensity, but not affective touch pleasantness. Therefore, the S1 is not directly involved in processing socially affective touch pleasantness, but still plays a role in discriminating touch location and intensity.[22]

Individual variation

A variety of studies have measured and investigated the causes for differences between individuals in the sense of fine touch. One well-studied area is passive tactile spatial acuity, the ability to resolve the fine spatial details of an object pressed against the stationary skin. A variety of methods have been used to measure passive tactile spatial acuity, perhaps the most rigorous being the grating orientation task.[23] In this task subjects identify the orientation of a grooved surface presented in two different orientations,[24] which can be applied manually or with automated equipment.[25] Many studies have shown a decline in passive tactile spatial acuity with age;[26][27][28] the reasons for this decline are unknown, but may include loss of tactile receptors during normal aging. Remarkably, index finger passive tactile spatial acuity is better among adults with smaller index fingertips;[29] this effect of finger size has been shown to underlie the better passive tactile spatial acuity of women, on average, compared to men.[29] The density of tactile corpuscles, a type of mechanoreceptor that detects low-frequency vibrations, is greater in smaller fingers;[30] the same may hold for Merkel cells, which detect the static indentations important for fine spatial acuity.[29] Among children of the same age, those with smaller fingers also tend to have better tactile acuity.[31] Many studies have shown that passive tactile spatial acuity is enhanced among blind individuals compared to sighted individuals of the same age,[28][32][33][34][35] possibly because of cross modal plasticity in the cerebral cortex of blind individuals. Perhaps also due to cortical plasticity, individuals who have been blind since birth reportedly consolidate tactile information more rapidly than sighted people.[36]

Clinical significance

A somatosensory deficiency may be caused by a peripheral neuropathy involving peripheral nerves of the somatosensory system.

This may present as numbness or paresthesia.

Certain types of seizures are associated with the somatosensory system. Cortical injury may lead to loss of thermal sensation or the ability to discriminate pain. An aura involving thermal and painful sensations is a phenomenon known to precede the onset of an epileptic seizure or focal seizure. Another type of seizure, called a sensory Jacksonian seizure involves an abnormal, localizable, cutaneous sensation but does not have apparent stimulus. This sensation may progress along a limb or to adjacent cutaneous body areas, reflecting abnormal neuronal firing in the postcentral gyrus where an epileptic discharge is propagated. These episodes in which patients are consciously aware during a seizure have been useful for identifying problems associated with the somatosensory cortex. Patients can describe the nature of the seizure and how they feel during it.[37]

The absence of proprioception or two-point tactile discrimination on one side of the body suggests injury to the contralateral side of the primary somatosensory cortex. However, depending on the extent of the injury, damage can range in loss of proprioception of an individual limb or the entire body. A deficit known as cortical astereognosis of the receptive type describes an inability to make use of tactile sensory information for identifying objects placed in the hand. For example, if this type of injury effects the hand region in the primary somatosensory cortex for one cerebral hemisphere, a patient with closed eyes cannot perceive the position of the fingers on the contralateral hand and will not be able to identify objects such as keys or a cell phone if they are placed into that hand.[37]

Evaluation of any suspected disease of the somatosensory system is included in a neurological examination of the peripheral nervous system

Clinically, a tuning fork of 128 Hz is used for testing vibrations.

Society and culture

Haptic technology can provide touch sensation in virtual and real environments.[38] In the field of speech therapy, tactile feedback can be used to treat speech disorders.

See also

References

  1. Blausen Medical (2014) "Primary motor, somatosensory, auditory and visual cortex." Wikiversity
  2. O. Franzen, R. Johansson, L. Terenius (1996) Somesthesis and the Neurobiology of the Somatosensory Cortex
  3. Robles-De-La-Torre G (2006). "The Importance of the Sense of Touch in Virtual and Real Environments" (PDF). IEEE Multimedia. 13 (3): 24–30. doi:10.1109/MMUL.2006.69.
  4. Tillotson, Joanne. McCann, Stephanie. Kaplan’s Medical Flashcards. Apr. 02. 2013.
  5. Albertine, Kurt. Barron’s Anatomy Flash Cards
  6. "Sense of Touch - How Do We Feel? A Simple Explanation." HubPages. HubPages, 19 July 2014. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  7. Paré, Michel, and Catherine Behets. "Paucity of Presumptive Ruffini Corpuscles in the Index Finger Pad of Humans." Wiley Online Library. 10 Feb. 2003. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  8. Scheibert J, Leurent S, Prevost A, Debrégeas G (2009). "The role of fingerprints in the coding of tactile information probed with a biomimetic sensor". Science. 323 (5920): 1503–6. PMID 19179493. doi:10.1126/science.1166467.
  9. Biswas, Abhijit; Manivannan, M.; Srinivasan, Mandyam A. (2015). "Vibrotactile Sensitivity Threshold: Nonlinear Stochastic Mechanotransduction Model of the Pacinian Corpuscle". IEEE Transactions on Haptics. 8 (1): 102–113. PMID 25398183. doi:10.1109/TOH.2014.2369422.
  10. Paré, Michel, and Robert Elde. "The Meissner Corpuscle Revised: A Multiafferented Mechanoreceptor with Nociceptor Immunochemical Properties." JNeurosci. 15 Sept. 2001. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  11. Asaga, Emi, and Kenjiro Takemura. "Tactile Evaluation Based on Human Tactile Perception Mechanism." Science Direct. 01 Dec. 2013. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  12. Buccino, G., and F. Binkofski. "Action Observation Activates Premotor and Parietal Areas in a Somatotopic Manner: An FMRI Study." Wiley Online Library. Jan. 2001. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  13. Seelke, Adele M.H., and Jeffrey J. Padberg. "Topographic Maps within Brodmann's Area 5 of Macaque Monkeys." Oxford Journals. 2011. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  14. Menon, Vinod, and Lucina Q. Uddin. "Saliency, Switching, Attention and Control: A Network Model of Insula." Springer. 29 May 2010. Web. 28 Mar. 2016.
  15. Geyer, Stefan, and Axel Schleicher. "Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex: 1. Microstructural Organization and Interindividual Variability." Science Direct. July 1999. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  16. Disbrow, Elizabeth, and Evangelos Litinas. "Thalamocortical Connections of the Parietal Ventral Area (PV) and the Second Somatosensory Area (S2) in Macaque Monkeys." Cambridge Journals Online. June 2002. Web. 27 Mar. 2016.
  17. Saladin KS. Anatomy and Physiology 3rd edd. 2004. McGraw-Hill, New York.
  18. 1 2 Zukerman, Wendy. "Skin 'sees' the light to protect against sunshine". newscientist.com. New Scientist. Retrieved 2015-01-22.
  19. Proske, U; Gandevia, SC (Oct 2012). "The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force". Physiol Rev. 92 (4): 1651–97. PMID 23073629. doi:10.1152/physrev.00048.2011.
  20. Proske & Gandevia 2009 "The kinaesthetic senses" p.4144 "The three sources for limb position and movement representation are tactile, visual, and proprioceptive."
  21. Case, LK; Laubacher, CM; Olausson, H; Wang, B; Spagnolo, PA; Bushnell, MC. "Encoding of Touch Intensity But Not Pleasantness in Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex". J Neurosci. 36: 5850–60. PMC 4879201Freely accessible. PMID 27225773. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1130-15.2016.
  22. Case, LK; Laubacher, CM; Olausson, H; Wang, B; Spagnolo, PA; Bushnell, MC. "Encoding of Touch Intensity But Not Pleasantness in Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex". J Neurosci. 36: 5850–60. PMC 4879201Freely accessible. PMID 27225773. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1130-15.2016.
  23. Van Boven, R. W.; Johnson, K. O. (1 December 1994). "The limit of tactile spatial resolution in humans: Grating orientation discrimination at the lip, tongue, and finger". Neurology. 44 (12): 2361–2361. doi:10.1212/WNL.44.12.2361.
  24. Craig JC. (1999). "Grating orientation as a measure of tactile spatial acuity.". Somatosens Mot Res. 16 (3): 197–206. PMID 10527368. doi:10.1080/08990229970456.
  25. Goldreich, D; Wong, M; Peters, RM; Kanics, IM (Jun 3, 2009). "A Tactile Automated Passive-Finger Stimulator (TAPS).". Journal of visualized experiments (28). PMC 2726582Freely accessible. PMID 19578327. doi:10.3791/1374.
  26. Stevens, JC; Alvarez-Reeves, M; Dipietro, L; Mack, GW; Green, BG (2003). "Decline of tactile acuity in aging: a study of body site, blood flow, and lifetime habits of smoking and physical activity.". Somatosensory & motor research. 20 (3–4): 271–9. PMID 14675966. doi:10.1080/08990220310001622997.
  27. Manning, Hélène; Tremblay, FranÇois (2006). "Age differences in tactile pattern recognition at the fingertip". Somatosensory & Motor Research. 23 (3–4): 147–155. doi:10.1080/08990220601093460.
  28. 1 2 Goldreich, D; Kanics, IM (2003). "Tactile acuity is enhanced in blindness.". Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (8): 3439–45. PMID 12716952.
  29. 1 2 3 Peters, RM; Hackeman, E; Goldreich, D (2009). "Diminutive digits discern delicate details: fingertip size and the sex difference in tactile spatial acuity.". Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (50): 15756–61. PMC 3849661Freely accessible. PMID 20016091. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3684-09.2009.
  30. Dillon, YK; Haynes, J; Henneberg, M (2001). "The relationship of the number of Meissner's corpuscles to dermatoglyphic characters and finger size.". Journal of Anatomy. 199 (Pt 5): 577–84. PMC 1468368Freely accessible. PMID 11760888. doi:10.1046/j.1469-7580.2001.19950577.x.
  31. Peters, RM; Goldreich, D (2013). "Tactile spatial acuity in childhood: effects of age and fingertip size.". PLOS ONE. 8 (12): e84650. PMC 3891499Freely accessible. PMID 24454612. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0084650.
  32. Stevens, Joseph C.; Foulke, Emerson; Patterson, Matthew Q. (1996). "Tactile acuity, aging, and braille reading in long-term blindness.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 2 (2): 91–106. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.2.2.91.
  33. Van Boven, RW; Hamilton, RH; Kauffman, T; Keenan, JP; Pascual-Leone, A (2000). "Tactile spatial resolution in blind braille readers.". Neurology. 54 (12): 2230–6. PMID 10881245. doi:10.1212/wnl.54.12.2230.
  34. Goldreich, D; Kanics, IM (2006). "Performance of blind and sighted humans on a tactile grating detection task.". Perception & psychophysics. 68 (8): 1363–71. PMID 17378422. doi:10.3758/bf03193735.
  35. Wong, M; Gnanakumaran, V; Goldreich, D (2011). "Tactile spatial acuity enhancement in blindness: evidence for experience-dependent mechanisms.". Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (19): 7028–37. PMID 21562264. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6461-10.2011.
  36. Bhattacharjee, A; Ye, AJ; Lisak, JA; Vargas, MG; Goldreich, D (2010). "Vibrotactile masking experiments reveal accelerated somatosensory processing in congenitally blind braille readers.". Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (43): 14288–98. PMC 3449316Freely accessible. PMID 20980584. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1447-10.2010.
  37. 1 2 Augustine, James R. (2008). Human Neuroanatomy. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. p. 360. ISBN 978-0-12-068251-5.
  38. Gabriel Robles-De-La-Torre. "International Society for Haptics: Haptic technology, an animated explanation". Isfh.org. Retrieved 2010-02-26.

Further reading

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.