Synesthesia
Synesthesia | |
---|---|
How someone with synesthesia might perceive certain letters and numbers. Synesthetes see characters just as others do (in whichever color actually displayed) but may simultaneously perceive colors as associated with or evoked by each one. | |
Classification and external resources | |
Specialty | Psychiatry, Neurology |
ICD-10 | R44.8 |
MeSH | C562460 |
Synesthesia (also spelled synæsthesia or synaesthesia; from the Ancient Greek σύν syn, "together", and αἴσθησις aisthēsis, "sensation") is a perceptual phenomenon in which stimulation of one sensory or cognitive pathway leads to automatic, involuntary experiences in a second sensory or cognitive pathway.[1][2][3][4] People who report a lifelong history of such experiences are known as synesthetes.
In one common form of synesthesia, known as grapheme-color synesthesia or color-graphemic synesthesia, letters or numbers are perceived as inherently colored.[5][6] In spatial-sequence, or number form synesthesia, numbers, months of the year, or days of the week elicit precise locations in space (for example, 1980 may be "farther away" than 1990), or may appear as a three-dimensional map (clockwise or counterclockwise).[7][8] Synesthetic associations can occur in any combination and any number of senses or cognitive pathways.[9]
Little is known about how synesthesia develops. It has been suggested that synesthesia develops during childhood when children are intensively engaged with abstract concepts for the first time.[10] This hypothesis – referred to as semantic vacuum hypothesis – explains why the most common forms of synesthesia are grapheme-color, spatial sequence and number form. These are usually the first abstract concepts that educational systems require children to learn.
Only a few types of synesthesia have been scientifically evaluated.[11] Awareness of synesthetic perceptions varies from person to person.[12]
Difficulties have been recognized in adequately defining synesthesia.[13][14] Many different phenomena have been included in the term synesthesia ("union of the senses"), and in many cases the terminology seems to be inaccurate. A more accurate but significantly less common term may be ideasthesia.
Signs and symptoms
Some synesthetes often report that they were unaware their experiences were unusual until they realized other people did not have them, while others report feeling as if they had been keeping a secret their entire lives.[11] The automatic and ineffable nature of a synesthetic experience means that the pairing may not seem out of the ordinary. This involuntary and consistent nature helps define synesthesia as a real experience. Most synesthetes report that their experiences are pleasant or neutral, although, in rare cases, synesthetes report that their experiences can lead to a degree of sensory overload.[15]
Though often stereotyped in the popular media as a medical condition or neurological aberration, many synesthetes themselves do not perceive their synesthetic experiences as a handicap. To the contrary, some report it as a gift—an additional "hidden" sense—something they would not want to miss. Most synesthetes become aware of their distinctive mode of perception in their childhood. Some have learned how to apply their ability in daily life and work. Synesthetes have used their abilities in memorization of names and telephone numbers, mental arithmetic, and more complex creative activities like producing visual art, music, and theater.[11]
Despite the commonalities which permit definition of the broad phenomenon of synesthesia, individual experiences vary in numerous ways. This variability was first noticed early in synesthesia research.[16] Some synesthetes report that vowels are more strongly colored, while for others consonants are more strongly colored.[15] Self reports, interviews, and autobiographical notes by synesthetes demonstrate a great degree of variety in types of synesthesia, intensity of synesthetic perceptions, awareness of the perceptual discrepancies between synesthetes and non-synesthetes, and the ways synesthesia is used in work, creative processes, and daily life.[11][17]
Synesthetes are very likely to participate in creative activities.[18] It has been suggested that individual development of perceptual and cognitive skills, in addition to one's cultural environment, produces the variety in awareness and practical use of synesthetic phenomena.[12][17] Synesthesia may also give a memory advantage. In one study conducted by Julia Simner of the University of Edinburgh it was found that spatial sequence synesthetes have a built-in and automatic mnemonic reference. So the nonsynesthete will need to create a mnemonic device to remember a sequence (like dates in a diary), but the synesthete can simply reference their spatial visualizations.[19]
Types
There are two overall forms of synesthesia: projective synesthesia and associative synesthesia. People who project will see actual colors, forms, or shapes when stimulated, as is commonly accepted as synesthesia; associators will feel a very strong and involuntary connection between the stimulus and the sense that it triggers. For example, in the common form chromesthesia (sound to color) a projector may hear a trumpet and see an orange triangle in space while an associator might hear a trumpet and think very strongly that it sounds "orange".
Synesthesia can occur between nearly any two senses or perceptual modes, and at least one synesthete, Solomon Shereshevsky, experienced synesthesia that linked all five senses. Types of synesthesia are indicated by using the notation x → y, where x is the "inducer" or trigger experience, and y is the "concurrent" or additional experience. For example, perceiving letters and numbers (collectively called graphemes) as colored would be indicated as grapheme → color synesthesia. Similarly, when synesthetes see colors and movement as a result of hearing musical tones, it would be indicated as tone → (color, movement) synesthesia.
While nearly every logically possible combination of experiences can occur, several types are more common than others.
Grapheme-color synesthesia
In one of the most common forms of synesthesia, individual letters of the alphabet and numbers (collectively referred to as graphemes) are "shaded" or "tinged" with a color. While different individuals usually do not report the same colors for all letters and numbers, studies with large numbers of synesthetes find some commonalities across letters (e.g. A is likely to be red).[15]
Chromesthesia
Another common form of synesthesia is the association of sounds with colors. For some, everyday sounds such as doors opening, cars honking, or people talking can trigger seeing colors. For others, colors are triggered when musical notes or keys are being played. People with synesthesia related to music may also have perfect pitch because their ability to see/hear colors aids them in identifying notes or keys.[20]
The colors triggered by certain sounds, and any other synesthetic visual experiences, are referred to as photisms.
According to Richard Cytowic,[3] chromesthesia is "something like fireworks": voice, music, and assorted environmental sounds such as clattering dishes or dog barks trigger color and firework shapes that arise, move around, and then fade when the sound ends. Sound often changes the perceived hue, brightness, scintillation, and directional movement. Some individuals see music on a "screen" in front of their faces. For Deni Simon, music produces waving lines "like oscilloscope configurations – lines moving in color, often metallic with height, width and, most importantly, depth. My favorite music has lines that extend horizontally beyond the 'screen' area."
Individuals rarely agree on what color a given sound is. B flat might be orange for one person and blue for another. Composers Franz Liszt and Nikolai Rimsky-Korsakov famously disagreed on the colors of music keys.
Spatial sequence synesthesia
Those with spatial sequence synesthesia (SSS) tend to see numerical sequences as points in space. For instance, the number 1 might be farther away and the number 2 might be closer. People with SSS may have superior memories; in one study, they were able to recall past events and memories far better and in far greater detail than those without the condition. They also see months or dates in the space around them. Some people see time like a clock above and around them.[21][22]
Number form
A number form is a mental map of numbers that automatically and involuntarily appears whenever someone who experiences number forms thinks of numbers. Number forms were first documented and named in 1881 by Francis Galton in "The Visions of Sane Persons".[23]
Auditory-tactile synesthesia
In auditory-tactile synesthesia, certain sounds can induce sensations in parts of the body. For example, someone with auditory-tactile synesthesia may experience that hearing a specific word feels like touch in one specific part of the body or may experience that certain sounds can create a sensation in the skin without being touched. It is one of the least common forms of synesthesia.[24] However, some speculate that the common phenomenon of autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR), in which auditory stimuli or trigger words create a tingling sensation in the body without being touched, to be a form of auditory-tactile synesthesia, meaning that it could be more common than current statistics imply.
Ordinal linguistic personification
Ordinal-linguistic personification (OLP, or personification for short) is a form of synesthesia in which ordered sequences, such as ordinal numbers, week-day names, months and alphabetical letters are associated with personalities or genders (Simner & Hubbard 2006). For example, the number 2 might be a young boy with a short temper, or the letter G might be a busy mother with a kind face. Although this form of synesthesia was documented as early as the 1890s (Flournoy 1893; Calkins 1893) researchers have, until recently, paid little attention to this form (see History of synesthesia research). This form of synesthesia was named as OLP in the contemporary literature by Julia Simner and colleagues [25] although it is now also widely recognised by the term "sequence-personality" synaesthesia. Ordinal linguistic personification normally co-occurs with other forms of synesthesia such as grapheme-color synesthesia.
Misophonia
Misophonia is a neurological disorder in which negative experiences (anger, fright, hatred, disgust) are triggered by specific sounds. Richard Cytowic suggests that misophonia is related to, or perhaps a variety of, synesthesia.[26] Miren Edelstein and her colleagues have compared misophonia to synesthesia in terms of connectivity between different brain regions as well as specific symptoms. They formed the hypothesis that "a pathological distortion of connections between the auditory cortex and limbic structures could cause a form of sound-emotion synesthesia."[27]
Mirror-touch synesthesia
This is a rare form of synesthesia where individuals feel the same sensation that another person feels (such as touch). For instance, when such a synesthete observes someone being tapped on their shoulder, the synesthete involuntarily feels a tap on their own shoulder as well. People with this type of synesthesia have been shown to have higher empathy levels compared to the general population. This may be related to the so-called mirror neurons present in the motor areas of the brain, which have also been linked to empathy.[28]
Lexical-gustatory synesthesia
This is another rare form of synesthesia where certain tastes are experienced when hearing words. For example, the word basketball might taste like waffles. The documentary 'Derek Tastes Of Earwax' gets its name from this phenomenon, in references to pub owner James Wannerton who experiences this particular sensation whenever he hears the name spoken.[29][30] It is estimated that 0.2% of the population has this form of synesthesia.[31]
Spatio-temporal synesthesia
In the manner of number form synesthesia, the spatio-temporal synesthesia is a mental map of days, weeks, or months of the year. Individuals presenting this synesthesia type declare that they can "see the time", for example, in the form of a ribbon, ring, or circle. According to certain researches,[32] those individuals possess peculiar synaptic connections in their brain, allowing them to perceive time as a spatial construction.
Like other forms of synesthesia, spatio-temporal synesthesia is consistent in its occurrences; even when tested months later, a synesthete will report the same experiences that they previously did.[33]
Other forms
Other forms of synesthesia have been reported, but little has been done to analyze them scientifically. There are at least 80 different types of synesthesia.[34]
The common phenomenon of ASMR Is considered by some to be a type of auditory-tactile and visual-tactile synesthesia.
Less common types include but are not limited to:
- phoneme-color
- lexeme-color
- smell-color
- flavor-color
- month-flavor
- sound-flavor
- visual-tactile
- pain-color
- personality-color (commonly referred to as "auras").
Mechanism
As of 2015 the neurological correlates of synesthesia had not been established.[36]
Dedicated regions of the brain are specialized for given functions. Increased cross-talk between regions specialized for different functions may account for the many types of synesthesia. For example, the additive experience of seeing color when looking at graphemes might be due to cross-activation of the grapheme-recognition area and the color area called V4 (see figure).[35] This is supported by the fact that grapheme-color synesthetes are able to identify the color of a grapheme in their peripheral vision even when they cannot consciously identify the shape of the grapheme.[35]
An alternative possibility is disinhibited feedback, or a reduction in the amount of inhibition along normally existing feedback pathways.[37] Normally, excitation and inhibition are balanced. However, if normal feedback were not inhibited as usual, then signals feeding back from late stages of multi-sensory processing might influence earlier stages such that tones could activate vision. Cytowic and Eagleman find support for the disinhibition idea in the so-called acquired forms[3] of synesthesia that occur in non-synesthetes under certain conditions: temporal lobe epilepsy,[38] head trauma, stroke, and brain tumors. They also note that it can likewise occur during stages of meditation, deep concentration, sensory deprivation, or with use of psychedelics such as LSD or mescaline, and even, in some cases, marijuana.[3] However, synesthetes report that common stimulants, like caffeine and cigarettes do not affect the strength of their synesthesia, nor does alcohol.[3]:137–40
A very different theoretical approach to synesthesia is that based on ideasthesia. According to this account, synesthesia is a phenomenon mediated by the extraction of the meaning of the inducing stimulus. Thus, synesthesia may be fundamentally a semantic phenomenon. Therefore, to understand neural mechanisms of synesthesia the mechanisms of semantics and the extraction of meaning need to be understood better. This is a non-trivial issue because it is not only a question of a location in the brain at which meaning is "processed" but pertains also to the question of understanding—epitomized in e.g., the Chinese room problem. Thus, the question of the neural basis of synesthesia is deeply entrenched into the general mind–body problem and the problem of the explanatory gap.[39]
Genetics
The genetic mechanism of synesthesia has long been debated. Due to the prevalence of synesthesia among the first-degree relatives of synesthetes,[40] there is evidence that synesthesia might have a genetic basis, however the monozygotic twins case studies indicate there is an epigenetic component. Synesthesia might also be a oligogenic condition, with Locus heterogeneity, multiple forms of inheritance (including Mendelian in some cases), and continuous variation in gene expression.
Diagnostic criteria
Although often termed a "neurological condition," synesthesia is not listed in either the DSM-IV or the ICD since it most often does not interfere with normal daily functioning.[41] Indeed, most synesthetes report that their experiences are neutral or even pleasant.[15] Like perfect pitch, synesthesia is simply a difference in perceptual experience.
The simplest approach is test-retest reliability over long periods of time, using stimuli of color names, color chips, or a computer-screen color picker providing 16.7 million choices. Synesthetes consistently score around 90% on reliability of associations, even with years between tests.[1] In contrast, non-synesthetes score just 30–40%, even with only a few weeks between tests and a warning that they would be retested.[1]
Grapheme-color synesthetes, as a group, share significant preferences for the color of each letter (e.g. A tends to be red; O tends to be white or black; S tends to be yellow etc.)[15] Nonetheless, there is a great variety in types of synesthesia, and within each type, individuals report differing triggers for their sensations and differing intensities of experiences. This variety means that defining synesthesia in an individual is difficult, and the majority of synesthetes are completely unaware that their experiences have a name.[15]
Neurologist Richard Cytowic identifies the following diagnostic criteria for synesthesia in his first edition book. However, the criteria are different in the second book:[1][2][3]
- Synesthesia is involuntary and automatic.
- Synesthetic perceptions are spatially extended, meaning they often have a sense of "location." For example, synesthetes speak of "looking at" or "going to" a particular place to attend to the experience.
- Synesthetic percepts are consistent and generic (i.e. simple rather than pictorial).
- Synesthesia is highly memorable.
- Synesthesia is laden with affect.
Cytowic's early cases mainly included individuals whose synesthesia was frankly projected outside the body (e.g. on a "screen" in front of one's face). Later research showed that such stark externalization occurs in a minority of synesthetes. Refining this concept, Cytowic and Eagleman differentiated between "localizers" and "non-localizers" to distinguish those synesthetes whose perceptions have a definite sense of spatial quality from those whose perceptions do not.[3]
Prevalence
Synesthesia is found in at least 4.4% of the population, as a high estimate, which is equivalent to 1 in 23 people.[42] This study had also concluded that one common form of synesthesia—grapheme-color synesthesia (colored letters and numbers) – is found in more than one percent of the population, and this latter prevalence of graphemes-color synesthesia has now been independently verified in a yet larger sample.[43] Earlier estimates of the prevalence of synesthesia were based on "best-guess" estimations only (e.g. 1 in 250,000) or had limitations in their methodologies because they required synesthetes to refer themselves for study (e.g. 1 in 2000) and for this reason the authors of those studies had been moderate in their claims.[44] Also, some individuals will not self-classify as synesthetes because they do not realize that their perceptions are different from those of everyone else.[35]
The most common forms of synesthesia are those that trigger colors, and the most prevalent of all is day-color.[42] Also relatively common is grapheme-color synesthesia. We can think of "prevalence" both in terms of how common is synesthesia (or different forms of synesthesia) within the population, or how common are different forms of synesthesia within synesthetes. So within synesthetes, forms of synesthesia that trigger color also appear to be the most common forms of synesthesia with a prevalence rate of 86% within synesthetes.[42] In another study, music-color is also prevalent at 18–41%. Some of the rarest are reported to be auditory-tactile, mirror-touch, and lexical-gustatory.[45]
There is research to suggest that the likelihood of having synesthesia is greater in people with autism.[46]
History
The interest in colored hearing dates back to Greek antiquity, when philosophers asked if the color (chroia, what we now call timbre) of music was a quantifiable quality.[47] Isaac Newton proposed that musical tones and color tones shared common frequencies, as did Goethe in his book Theory of Colours.[48] There is a long history of building color organs such as the clavier à lumières on which to perform colored music in concert halls.[49][49][50]
The first medical description of "colored hearing" is in an 1812 thesis by the German physician Sachs.[51] The "father of psychophysics," Gustav Fechner, reported the first empirical survey of colored letter photisms among 73 synesthetes in 1876,[52][53] followed in the 1880s by Francis Galton.[7][54][55] Carl Jung refers to "color hearing" in his Symbols of Transformation in 1912.[56] Research into synesthesia proceeded briskly in several countries, but due to the difficulties in measuring subjective experiences and the rise of behaviorism, which made the study of any subjective experience taboo, synesthesia faded into scientific oblivion between 1930 and 1980.
As the 1980s cognitive revolution made inquiry into internal subjective states respectable again, scientists returned to synesthesia. Led in the United States by Larry Marks and Richard Cytowic, and later in England by Simon Baron-Cohen and Jeffrey Gray, researchers explored the reality, consistency, and frequency of synesthetic experiences. In the late 1990s, the focus settled on grapheme → color synesthesia, one of the most common[15] and easily studied types. Psychologists and neuroscientists study synesthesia not only for its inherent appeal, but also for the insights it may give into cognitive and perceptual processes that occur in synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike. Synesthesia is now the topic of scientific books and papers, PhD theses, documentary films, and even novels.
Since the rise of the Internet in the 1990s, synesthetes began contacting one another and creating web sites devoted to the condition. These rapidly grew into international organizations such as the American Synesthesia Association, the UK Synaesthesia Association, the Belgian Synaesthesia Association, the Canadian Synesthesia Association, the German Synesthesia Association, and the Netherlands Synesthesia Web Community.
Society and culture
Notable cases
As mentioned before, one of the most notable synesthetes is Solomon Shereshevsky, a newspaper reporter turned celebrated mnemonist, who was discovered by Russian neuropsychologist, Alexander Luria, to have a rare fivefold form of synesthesia.[57] Words and text were not only associated with highly vivid visuo-spatial imagery but also sound, taste, color, and sensation.[57] Shereshevsky could recount endless details of many things without form, from lists of names to decades-old conversations, but he had great difficulty grasping abstract concepts. The automatic, and nearly permanent, retention of every little detail due to synesthesia greatly inhibited Shereshevsky from understanding much of what he read or heard.[57]
Ramachandran, a neuroscientist and author, studied the case of a grapheme-color synesthete who was also color blind. While he couldn't see certain colors with his eyes, he could still "see" those colors when looking at certain letters. Because he didn't have a name for those colors, he called them "martian colors."[58]
Art
Other notable synesthetes come particularly from artistic professions and backgrounds. Synesthetic art historically refers to multi-sensory experiments in the genres of visual music, music visualization, audiovisual art, abstract film, and intermedia.[11][59][60][61][62][63] Distinct from neuroscience, the concept of synesthesia in the arts is regarded as the simultaneous perception of multiple stimuli in one gestalt experience.[64]
Neurological synesthesia has been a source of inspiration for artists, composers, poets, novelists, and digital artists. Vladimir Nabokov writes explicitly about synesthesia in several novels. Wassily Kandinsky (a synesthete) and Piet Mondrian (not a synesthete) both experimented with image-music congruence in their paintings. Alexander Scriabin composed colored music that was deliberately contrived and based on the circle of fifths, whereas Olivier Messiaen invented a new method of composition (the modes of limited transposition) specifically to render his bi-directional sound-color synesthesia. For example, the red rocks of Bryce Canyon are depicted in his symphony Des canyons aux étoiles... ("From the Canyons to the Stars"). New art movements such as literary symbolism, non-figurative art, and visual music have profited from experiments with synesthetic perception and contributed to the public awareness of synesthetic and multi-sensory ways of perceiving.[11]
Contemporary artists with synesthesia, such as Carol Steen[65] and Marcia Smilack[66] (a photographer who waits until she gets a synesthetic response from what she sees and then takes the picture), use their synesthesia to create their artwork. Brandy Gale, a Canadian visual artist, experiences an involuntary joining or crossing of any of her senses – hearing, vision, taste, touch, smell and movement. Gale paints from life rather than from photographs and by exploring the sensory panorama of each locale attempts to capture, select, and transmit these personal experiences.[67][68][69]
David Hockney perceives music as color, shape, and configuration and uses these perceptions when painting opera stage sets (though not while creating his other artworks). Kandinsky combined four senses: color, hearing, touch, and smell.[1][3] Nabokov described his grapheme-color synesthesia at length in his autobiography, Speak, Memory, and portrayed it in some of his characters.[70] In addition to Messiaen, whose three types of complex colors are rendered explicitly in musical chord structures that he invented,[3][71] other composers who reported synesthesia include Duke Ellington,[72] Rimsky-Korsakov,[73] and Jean Sibelius.[60] Michael Torke is a contemporary example of a synesthetic composer.[60] Physicist Richard Feynman describes his colored equations in his autobiography, What Do You Care What Other People Think?[74]
Other notable synesthetes include musicians Billy Joel,[75]:89, 91 Itzhak Perlman,[75]:53 Alexander Frey, Ida Maria,[76] Brian Chase[77][78] and Patrick Stump; actress Stephanie Carswell (credited as Stéphanie Montreux); inventor Nikola Tesla;[79] electronic musician Richard D. James aka Aphex Twin (who claims to be inspired by lucid dreams as well as music); and classical pianist Hélène Grimaud. Drummer Mickey Hart of The Grateful Dead wrote about his experiences with synaesthesia in his autobiography Drumming at the Edge of Magic.[80] Pharrell Williams, of the groups The Neptunes and N.E.R.D., also experiences synesthesia[81][82] and used it as the basis of the album Seeing Sounds. Singer/songwriter Marina and the Diamonds experiences music → color synesthesia and reports colored days of the week.[83]
Some artists frequently mentioned as synesthetes did not, in fact, have the neurological condition. Scriabin's 1911 Prometheus, for example, is a deliberate contrivance whose color choices are based on the circle of fifths and appear to have been taken from Madame Blavatsky.[3][84] The musical score has a separate staff marked luce whose "notes" are played on a color organ. Technical reviews appear in period volumes of Scientific American.[3] On the other hand, his older colleague Rimsky-Korsakov (who was perceived as a fairly conservative composer) was, in fact, a synesthete.[85]
French poets Arthur Rimbaud and Charles Baudelaire wrote of synesthetic experiences, but there is no evidence they were synesthetes themselves. Baudelaire's 1857 Correspondances introduced the notion that the senses can and should intermingle. Baudelaire participated in a hashish experiment by psychiatrist Jacques-Joseph Moreau and became interested in how the senses might affect each other.[11] Rimbaud later wrote Voyelles (1871), which was perhaps more important than Correspondances in popularizing synesthesia. He later boasted "J'inventais la couleur des voyelles!" (I invented the colors of the vowels!).[86]
Daniel Tammet wrote a book on his experiences with synesthesia called Born on a Blue Day.[87]
Joanne Harris, author of Chocolat, is a synesthete who says she experiences colors as scents.[88] Her novel Blueeyedboy features various aspects of synesthesia.
Literature
Synesthesia is sometimes used as a plot device or way of developing a character's inner life. Author and synesthete Pat Duffy describes five ways in which synesthetic characters have been used in modern fiction.[89][90]
- Synesthesia as Romantic ideal: in which the condition illustrates the Romantic ideal of transcending one's experience of the world. Books in this category include The Gift by Vladimir Nabokov.
- Synesthesia as pathology: in which the trait is pathological. Books in this category include The Whole World Over by Julia Glass.
- Synesthesia as Romantic pathology: in which synesthesia is pathological but also provides an avenue to the Romantic ideal of transcending quotidian experience. Books in this category include Holly Payne’s The Sound of Blue.
- Synesthesia as psychological health and balance: Painting Ruby Tuesday by Jane Yardley, and A Mango-Shaped Space by Wendy Mass.
- Synesthesia as young adult literature and science fiction: Ultraviolet by R. J. Anderson, and One Is Not A Lonely Number by Evelyn Krieger.
- Synesthesia suffered by character Amos Decker in the popular fictional detective series Memory Man by David Baldacci.
Many literary depictions of synesthesia are not accurate. Some say more about an author's interpretation of synesthesia than the phenomenon itself.
Research
Research on synesthesia raises questions about how the brain combines information from different sensory modalities, referred to as crossmodal perception or multisensory integration.
An example of this is the bouba/kiki effect. In an experiment first designed by Wolfgang Köhler, people are asked to choose which of two shapes is named bouba and which kiki. 95% to 98% of people choose kiki for the angular shape and bouba for the rounded one. Individuals on the island of Tenerife showed a similar preference between shapes called takete and maluma. Even 2.5-year-old children (too young to read) show this effect.[91] Recent research indicated that in the background of this effect may operate a form of ideasthesia.[92]
Researchers hope that the study of synesthesia will provide better understanding of consciousness and its neural correlates. In particular, synesthesia might be relevant to the philosophical problem of qualia,[4][93] given that synesthetes experience extra qualia (e.g. colored sound). An important insight for qualia research may come from the findings that synesthesia has the properties of ideasthesia,[13] which then suggest a crucial role of conceptualization processes in generating qualia.[10]
Technological applications
Synesthesia also has a number of practical applications, one of which is the use of 'intentional synesthesia' in technology.[94]
The Voice
Peter Meijer developed a sensory substitution device called The vOICe (the capital letters "O," "I," and "C" in "vOICe" are intended to evoke the expression "Oh I see"). The vOICe is a privately owned research project, running without venture capital, that was first implemented using low-cost hardware in 1991.[95] The vOICe is a visual-to-auditory sensory substitution device (SSD) preserving visual detail at high resolution (up to 25,344 pixels).[96] The device consists of a laptop, head-mounted camera or computer camera, and headphones. The vOICe converts visual stimuli of the surroundings captured by the camera into corresponding aural representations (soundscapes) delivered to the user through headphones at a default rate of one soundscape per second. Each soundscape is a left-to-right scan, with height represented by pitch, and brightness by loudness.[97] The vOICe compensates for the loss of vision by converting information from the lost sensory modality into stimuli in a remaining modality.[98]
See also
References
- 1 2 3 4 5 Cytowic, Richard E. (2002). Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses (2nd edition). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-03296-1. OCLC 49395033.
- 1 2 Cytowic, Richard E. (2003). The Man Who Tasted Shapes. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-53255-7. OCLC 53186027.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 Cytowic, Richard E; Eagleman, David M (2009). Wednesday is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia (with an afterword by Dmitri Nabokov). Cambridge: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-01279-0.
- 1 2 Harrison, John E.; Simon Baron-Cohen (1996). Synaesthesia: classic and contemporary readings. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. ISBN 0-631-19764-8. OCLC 59664610.
- ↑ Rich AN, Mattingley JB (January 2002). "Anomalous perception in synesthesia: a cognitive neuroscience perspective". Nature Reviews Neuroscience (Review). 3 (1): 43–52. PMID 11823804. doi:10.1038/nrn702.
- ↑ Hubbard EM, Ramachandran VS (November 2005). "Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia" (PDF). Neuron (Review). 48 (3): 509–20. PMID 16269367. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.012.
- 1 2 3 Galton F (1880). "Visualized Numerals". Nature. 21 (543): 494–5. doi:10.1038/021494e0.
- ↑ Seron X, Pesenti M, Noël MP, Deloche G, Cornet JA (August 1992). "Images of numbers, or "When 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue"". Cognition. 44 (1–2): 159–96. PMID 1511585. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90053-K.
- ↑ "How Synesthesia Works". HowStuffWorks. 1970-01-01. Retrieved 2016-05-02.
- 1 2 Mroczko-Wąsowicz A.; Nikolić D. (2014). "Semantic mechanisms may be responsible for developing synesthesia". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 509. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00509.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 van Campen, Cretien (2007). The Hidden Sense: Synesthesia in Art and Science. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-22081-4. OCLC 80179991.
- 1 2 Campen, Cretien van (2009) "The Hidden Sense: On Becoming Aware of Synesthesia" TECCOGS, vol. 1, pp. 1–13."Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 July 2009. Retrieved 18 February 2009.
- 1 2 Nikolić D (2009). "Is synaesthesia actually ideaesthesia? An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon" (PDF). Proceedings of the Third International Congress on Synaesthesia, Science & Art, Granada, Spain, April 26–29.
- ↑ Simner J (2012). "Defining synaesthesia". British Journal of Psychology (Review). 103 (6): 1–15. PMID 22229768. doi:10.1348/000712610X528305.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Sagiv, Noam; Robertson, Lynn C (2005). Synesthesia: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-516623-X. OCLC 53020292.
- ↑ Flournoy, Théodore (2001). Des phénomènes de synopsie (Audition colorée). Adamant Media Corporation. ISBN 0-543-94462-X.
- 1 2 Dittmar, A. (Ed.) (2007) Synästhesien. Roter Faden durchs Leben? Essen, Verlag Die Blaue Eule.
- ↑ Dailey A, Martindale C, Borkum J (1997). "Creativity, synesthesia, and physiognomic perception". Creativity Research Journal. 10 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1001_1.
- ↑ "Investigating Spatial Sequence Synesthesia". Synesthesia Test. 2012-06-26. Retrieved 2016-05-02.
- ↑ "Absolute Pitch and Synesthesia | The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research". The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research. Retrieved 2016-05-02.
- ↑ Do sequence-space synaesthetes have better spatial imagery skills? Maybe not, The National Center for Biotechnology Information
- ↑ A Mind That Touches the Past, Sciencemag.org
- ↑ Galton F (1881). "The visions of sane persons" (PDF). Fortnightly Review. 29: 729–40. Retrieved 2008-06-17.
- ↑ Naumer MJ, van den Bosch JJ (July 2009). "Touching sounds: thalamocortical plasticity and the neural basis of multisensory integration". J. Neurophysiol. 102 (1): 7–8. PMID 19403745. doi:10.1152/jn.00209.2009.
- ↑ Simner J, Hubbard EM (December 2006). "Variants of synesthesia interact in cognitive tasks: evidence for implicit associations and late connectivity in cross-talk theories". Neuroscience. 143: 805–14. PMID 16996695. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.018.
- ↑ Cytowic, Richard E. (2002). Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses (2nd edition). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-03296-1. OCLC 49395033
- ↑ Edelstein Miren; Brang David; Rouw Romke; Ramachandran Vilayanur S (2013). "Misophonia: Physiological Investigations and Case Descriptions". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7. PMC 3691507 . PMID 23805089. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00296.
- ↑ Heyes, Cecelia. "Where Do Mirror Neurons Come From?" Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (2009): 576–77. CognitiveScience. University of Oxford, 7 November 2009. Web. 30 January 2015.
- ↑ "Derek Tastes of Ear Wax". Top Documentary Films. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
- ↑ "BBC – Science & Nature – Horizon". bbc.co.uk. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
- ↑ Simner, Julia (2009). Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Springer Berlin Heidelberg. ISBN 978-3-540-23735-8.
- ↑ "Les Rares Humains Qui Vioent le Temps et ont des Souvenirs" (in French).
- ↑ "Your Mental Image of Time". Archived from the original on 4 March 2016.
- ↑ "Types-of-Syn". www.daysyn.com. Retrieved 2016-01-26.
- 1 2 3 4 5 Ramachandran VS, Hubbard EM (2001). "Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language" (PDF). Journal of Consciousness Studies. pp. 3–34. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 May 2006.
- ↑ Hupé JM, Dojat M (2015). "A critical review of the neuroimaging literature on synesthesia". Front Hum Neurosci. 9: 103. PMC 4379872 . PMID 25873873. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00103.
- ↑ Grossenbacher P. G.; Lovelace C. T. (January 2001). "Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints". Trends Cogn. Sci. 5 (1): 36–41. PMID 11164734. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01571-0.
- ↑ Marcel Neckar; Petr Bob (11 January 2016). "Synesthetic associations and psychosensory symptoms of temporal epilepsy". Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. National Institutes of Health (NIH). 12: 109–12. PMC 4714732 . PMID 26811683. doi:10.2147/NDT.S95464.
- ↑ Gray JA, Parslow DM, Brammer MJ, Chopping S, Vythelingum GN, Fytche DH (2006). "Evidence Against Functionalism from Neuroimaging of the Alien Colour Effect in Synaesthesia". Cortex. 42 (2): 317.
- ↑ Baron-Cohen, Simon; Burt, Lucy; Smith-Laittan, Fiona; Harrison, John; Bolton, Patrick (1 September 1996). "Synaesthesia: prevalence and familiality". Perception. 25 (9): 1073–1079. doi:10.1068/p251073. Retrieved 4 April 2017.
- ↑ Hubbard, Edward (June 2007). "Neurophysiology of synesthesia". Curr Psychiatry Rep. 9: 193–9. PMID 17521514. doi:10.1007/s11920-007-0018-6.
- 1 2 3 Simner J, Mulvenna C, Sagiv N, Tsakanikos E, Witherby SA, Fraser C, Scott K, Ward J (2006). "Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences" (PDF). Perception. 35: 1024–1033. PMID 17076063. doi:10.1068/p5469. Retrieved 26 April 2016.
- ↑ Carmichael DA, Down MP, Shillcock RC, Eagleman DM, Simner J (2015). "Validating a standardised test battery for synesthesia: Does the Synesthesia Battery reliably detect synesthesia". Conscious Cogn. 33: 375–85. PMID 25734257. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.001.
- ↑ http://pec.sagepub.com/content/25/9/1073.short. Missing or empty
|title=
(help) - ↑ Safran, Avinoam; Sanda, Nicolae (24 December 2014). "Colour synesthesia. Insight into perception, emotion, and consciousness". Current Opinion in Neurology. 28 (1): 36–44. PMC 4286234 . PMID 25545055. doi:10.1097/WCO.0000000000000169.
- ↑ Baron-Cohen S, Johnson D, Asher J, Wheelwright S, Fisher SE, Gregerson PK, Allison C, "Is synaesthesia more common in autism?", Molecular Autism, 20 November 2013
- ↑ Gage, J.Colour and Culture. Practice and Meaning from Antiquity to Abstraction. (London:Thames & Hudson, 1993).
- ↑ Goethe, Johann Wolfgang (1840). Theory of Colours. J. Murray.
- 1 2 Peacock, Kenneth. "Instruments to Perform Color-Music: Two Centuries of Technological Experimentation,"Leonardo 21, No. 4 (1988) 397–406.
- ↑ Jewanski, J. & N. Sidler (Eds.). Farbe – Licht – Musik. Synaesthesie und Farblichtmusik. Bern: Peter Lang, 2006.
- ↑ Mahling F (1926). "Das Problem der 'audition colorée': Eine historisch-kritische Untersuchung". Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie. 57: 165–301.
- ↑ Fechner, G. (1876) Vorschule der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel. Website:
- ↑ Campen Cretien van (1996). "De verwarring der zintuigen. Artistieke en psychologische experimenten met synesthesie". Psychologie & Maatschappij. 20 (1): 10–26.
- ↑ Galton F (1880). "Visualized Numerals". Nature. 21 (533): 252–6. doi:10.1038/021252a0.
- ↑ Galton F (1883). Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development. Macmillan. Retrieved 2008-06-17.
- ↑ Jung, C.G. The Tansformation of Libido in "Symbols of Transformation", CW5, London 1912/1956, Routledge & Kegan Paul, para.237.
- 1 2 3 Schacter, Daniel (1996). Searching for Memory: The Brain, The Mind, And The Past. Basic Books. p. 81. ISBN 0465075525.
- ↑ http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/cosmicvariance/2007/11/05/martian-colors/#.WAPZ7ZMrKu4
- ↑ Campen C (1999). "Artistic and psychological experiments with synesthesia". Leonardo. 32 (1): 9–14. doi:10.1162/002409499552948.
- 1 2 3 Berman G (1999). "Synesthesia and the Arts". Leonardo. 32 (1): 15–22. doi:10.1162/002409499552957.
- ↑ Maur, Karin von (1999). The Sound of Painting: Music in Modern Art (Pegasus Library). Munich: Prestel. ISBN 3-7913-2082-3.
- ↑ Gage, John D. (1993). Colour and culture: practice and meaning from antiquity to abstraction. London: Thames and Hudson. ISBN 0-500-27818-0.
- ↑ Gage, John D. (1999). Color and meaning: art, science, and symbolism. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-22611-9.
- ↑ Campen, Cretien van (2009) Visual Music and Musical Paintings. The Quest for Synesthesia in the Arts. In: F. Bacci & D. Melcher. Making Sense of Art, making Art of Sense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- ↑ Steen C (2001). "Visions Shared: A Firsthand Look into Synesthesia and Art". Leonardo. 34 (3): 203–208. doi:10.1162/002409401750286949.
- ↑ Marcia Smilack Website Accessed 20 August 2006.
- ↑ "Coastal Synaesthesia: Paintings and Photographs of Hawaii, Fiji and California by Brandy Gale – Gualala Arts Center exhibit: January, 2015". gualalaarts.org. Archived from the original on 2 February 2015. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
- ↑ EG. "The Wondrous Sensory Spectrum of Brandy Gale". FORA.tv. Retrieved 2 February 2015.
- ↑ brandygale
.com - ↑ Nabokov, Vladimir. 1966. Speak, Memory: An Autobiography Revisited. New York: Putnam.
- ↑ see Samuel, Claude. 1994 (1986). Olivier Messiaen: Music and Color. Conversations with Claude Samuel. Translated by E. Thomas Glasow. Portland, Oregon: Amadeus Press.
- ↑ Ellington, as quoted in George, Don. 1981. Sweet man: The real Duke Ellington. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons. Page 226.
- ↑ according to the Russian press: Yastrebtsev V. "On N.A.Rimsky-Korsakov's color sound- contemplation." Russkaya muzykalnaya gazeta, 1908, N 39–40, p. 842–845 (in Russian), cited by Bulat Galeyev (1999).
- ↑ Feynman, Richard. 1988. What Do You Care What Other People Think? New York: Norton. P. 59.
- 1 2 Seaberg, M. (2011). Tasting the Universe. New Page Books. ISBN 978-1-60163-159-6.
- ↑ Cairns, Dan (24 February 2008). "Times Online interview". The Times. London. Retrieved 2008-07-24.
- ↑ Forrest, Emma (30 March 2009). "Emma Forrest meets New York's favourite art-punk rockers Yeah Yeah Yeahs". guardian.co.uk. London: The Guardian. Retrieved 2009-05-07.
- ↑ Chase, Brian. "Brian Chase's blog". yeahyeahyeahs.com. Archived from the original on 25 January 2009. Retrieved 2009-05-07.
- ↑ Tesla, Nikola. "The Strange Life of Nikola Tesla" (PDF). pitt.edu. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 July 2012. Retrieved 4 September 2012.
- ↑ Hart, M., Stevens, J., & Lieberman, F. (1990). Drumming at the edge of magic: a journey into the spirit of percussion. San Francisco, CA: HarperSanFrancisco. pp.133
- ↑ It just always stuck out in my mind, and I could always see it. I don't know if that makes sense, but I could always visualize what I was hearing... Yeah, it was always like weird colors." From a Nightline interview with Pharrell
- ↑ "Synesthetes: "People of the Future"". Psychology Today. 3 March 2012. Retrieved 15 May 2014.
- ↑ Loose Women | Marina and the Diamonds – ITV Lifestyle ITV – 27 April 2010 – Retrieved 28 April 2010.
- ↑ Dann, Kevin T. (1998). Bright colors falsely seen: synaesthesia and the search for transcendental knowledge. New Haven, Conn: Yale University Press. ISBN 0-300-06619-8.
- ↑ This is according to an article in the Russian press, Yastrebtsev V. "On N.A.Rimsky-Korsakov's color sound- contemplation." Russkaya muzykalnaya gazeta, 1908, N 39–40, pp. 842–845 (in Russian), cited by Bulat Galeyev (1999).
- ↑ Barth, Friedrich; Giampieri-Deutsch, Patrizia; Hans-Dieter, Klein (2012). Sensory Perception: Mind & Matter. Vienna: Springer Vienna. p. 221. ISBN 9783211997505.
I invented the colours of the vowels!
- ↑ Tammet, Daniel (2007). Born on a Blue Day. Free Press. ISBN 978-1416535072.
- ↑ "Chocolat author Joanne Harris talks about her latest novel Blue Eyed Boy". Metro. 7 April 2010.
- ↑ Duffy, P.L. (2006). Images of Synesthetes and their Perceptions of Language in Fiction. 6th Annual Meeting of the American Synesthesia Association. University of South Florida. Archived from the original on 17 January 2012.
- ↑ Duffy PL, Simner J (2010). "Synaesthesia in fiction". Cortex. 46 (2): 277–278. PMID 19081086. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.11.003.
- ↑ Maurer D, Pathman T, Mondloch CJ (May 2006). "The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults". Dev Sci. 9 (3): 316–22. PMID 16669803. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x.
- ↑ Gómez Milán E.; Iborra O.; de Córdoba M.J.; Juárez-Ramos V.; Artacho Rodríguez; Rubio J.L. (2013). "The Kiki-Bouba effect: A case of personification and ideaesthesia". The Journal of Consciousness Studies. 20 (1–2): 84–102.
- ↑ Gray JA, Chopping S, Nunn J, et al. (2002). "Implications of synaesthesia for functionalism: Theory and experiments". Journal of Consciousness. 9 (12): 5–31.
- ↑ Suslick, Kenneth S (December 2012). "Synesthesia in science and technology: more than making the unseen visible". Current Opinion in Chemical Biology. 16 (5–6): 557–563. PMC 3606019 . PMID 23183411. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2012.10.030.
- ↑ Meijer, Peter. "Augmented Reality for the Totally Blind". Retrieved 4 February 2014.
- ↑ Striem-Amit, Ella; Guendelman, Miriam; Amedi, Amir; Serino, Andrea (16 March 2012). "'Visual' Acuity of the Congenitally Blind Using Visual-to-Auditory Sensory Substitution". PLoS ONE. 7 (3): e33136. PMC 3306374 . PMID 22438894. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033136.
- ↑ Carmichael, Joey. "Device Trains Blind People To 'See' By Listening". Retrieved 4 February 2014.
- ↑ Haigh, Alastair; Brown, David J.; Meijer, Peter; Proulx, Michael J. (2013). "How well do you see what you hear? The acuity of visual-to-auditory sensory substitution". Frontiers in Psychology. 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00330.
Further reading
- de Broucker T (April 2013). "Synaesthesia, an augmented sensory world: phenomenology and literature review". Rev. Neurol. (Paris) (Review) (in French). 169 (4): 328–34. PMID 23434143. doi:10.1016/j.neurol.2012.09.016.
- Cohen Kadosh R, Terhune DB (February 2012). "Redefining synaesthesia?". Br J Psychol (Review). 103 (1): 20–3. PMID 22229770. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02003.x.
- De Cordoba, M.J.; Riccò, D.; Day, Sean (July 2014). Synaesthesia: Theoretical, artistic and scientific foundations. Granada, Spain. p. 372. ISBN 978-84-939054-9-1.
- Dael N, Sierro G, Mohr C (2013). "Affect-related synesthesias: a prospective view on their existence, expression and underlying mechanisms". Front Psychol (Review). 4: 754. PMC 3798864 . PMID 24151478. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00754.
- Fitzgibbon BM, Enticott PG, Rich AN, Giummarra MJ, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Bradshaw JL (January 2012). "Mirror-sensory synaesthesia: exploring 'shared' sensory experiences as synaesthesia". Neurosci Biobehav Rev (Review). 36 (1): 645–57. PMID 21986634. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.09.006.
- Luke DP, Terhune DB (2013). "The induction of synaesthesia with chemical agents: a systematic review". Front Psychol (Review). 4: 753. PMC 3797969 . PMID 24146659. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00753.
- Mylopoulos MI, Ro T (2013). "Synesthesia: a colorful word with a touching sound?". Front Psychol (Review). 4: 763. PMC 3804765 . PMID 24155733. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00763.
- Price MC, Mattingley JB (May 2013). "Automaticity in sequence-space synaesthesia: a critical appraisal of the evidence". Cortex (Review). 49 (5): 1165–86. PMID 23237480. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.013.
- Rothen N, Meier B, Ward J (September 2012). "Enhanced memory ability: Insights from synaesthesia". Neurosci Biobehav Rev (Review). 36 (8): 1952–63. PMID 22634573. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.004.
- Sinke C, Halpern JH, Zedler M, Neufeld J, Emrich HM, Passie T (September 2012). "Genuine and drug-induced synesthesia: a comparison". Conscious Cogn (Review). 21 (3): 1419–34. PMID 22521474. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.03.009.
- Simner J (February 2012). "Defining synaesthesia". Br J Psychol (Review). 103 (1): 1–15. PMID 22229768. doi:10.1348/000712610X528305.
- Suslick KS (December 2012). "Synesthesia in science and technology: more than making the unseen visible". Curr Opin Chem Biol (Review). 16 (5–6): 557–63. PMC 3606019 . PMID 23183411. doi:10.1016/j.cbpa.2012.10.030.
- Ward J (2013). "Synesthesia". Annu Rev Psychol (Review). 64: 49–75. PMID 22747246. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143840.
External links
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Synesthesia. |
- Red Mondays and Gemstone Jalapeños: The Synesthetic World a documentary short featuring, featuring David Eagleman and four synesthetes, from ResearchChannel.
- Danis, Alex. "Grapheme → colour synesthesia". Numberphile. Brady Haran.
- Mailman, Joshua B. "Improvising Synesthesia: Comprovisation of Generative Graphics and Music" in Leonardo Electronic Almanac v.19 no.3, Live Visuals, 2013, pp. 352–84.
- Palmer, Stephen; Schloss, Karen B. (21 August 2015). "What's the Color of Your Favorite Song? – The Crux". Discover Magazine Blogs. Kalmbach Publishing Co.
- "Synesthesia". Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.