Rhoticity in English
History and description of |
English pronunciation |
---|
Historical stages |
General development |
Development of vowels |
Development of consonants |
Variable features |
Related topics |
Rhoticity in English refers to the situations in which English speakers pronounce the historical rhotic consonant /r/, and is one of the most prominent distinctions by which varieties of English can be classified. The English dialects of Scotland, Ireland, and most of the United States and Canada preserve historical /r/, and are thus termed the "rhotic varieties".[lower-alpha 1] The historical /r/ is not pronounced except before vowels in "non-rhotic varieties";[lower-alpha 2] these include all the dialects of modern England except the South West, the southern West Midlands, and parts of west Lancashire, as well as the English dialects of Australia, New Zealand, South Africa, and some parts of the southern and eastern coastal United States.[1]
In non-rhotic varieties, speakers no longer pronounce /r/ in postvocalic environments—that is, when it is immediately after a vowel and not followed by another vowel.[4][1] For example, a rhotic English speaker pronounces the words hard and butter as /ˈhɑːrd/ and /ˈbʌtər/, whereas a non-rhotic speaker "drops" or "deletes" the /r/ sound, pronouncing them as /ˈhɑːd/ and /ˈbʌtə/. A non-rhotic speaker usually still pronounces the /r/ in the phrase "butter and jam" (the linking R), since the /r/ is followed by a vowel in this case.
Evidence from written documents suggests that loss of postvocalic /r/ began sporadically during the mid-15th century. However, these /r/-less spellings were uncommon and were restricted to private documents, especially ones written by women. In the mid-18th century, postvocalic /r/ was still pronounced in most environments, but by the 1740s to 1770s it was often deleted entirely, especially after low vowels. By the 1790s, fully non-rhotic pronunciation had become common in London and surrounding areas, and was being increasingly used even in more formal and educated speech. By the early 19th century, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though some variation persisted as late as the 1870s. This loss of postvocalic /r/ in British English influenced southern and eastern American port cities with close connections to Britain, causing their upper-class pronunciation to become non-rhotic while the rest of the United States remained rhotic. Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American prestige speech until the 1860s, when the American Civil War shifted America's centers of wealth and political power to rhotic areas with fewer cultural connections to the old colonial and British elites. The advent of radio and television in the early 20th century established a national standard of American pronunciation that fully preserved historical /r/.
History
The earliest traces of a loss of /r/ in English appear in the early 15th century and occur before coronal consonants, especially /s/, giving modern "ass (buttocks)" (Old English ears, Middle English ers or ars), and "bass (fish)" (OE bærs, ME bars).[1] A second phase of /r/-loss began during the 15th century, and was characterized by sporadic and lexically-variable deletion, such as monyng "morning" and cadenall "cardinal".[1] These /r/-less spellings appear throughout the 16th and 17th centuries, but are uncommon and are restricted to private documents, especially ones written by women.[1] No English authorities describe loss of /r/ in the standard language prior to the mid-18th century, and many do not fully accept it until the 1790s.[1]
During the mid-17th century, a number of sources describe /r/ as being weakened but still present.[7] The English playwright Ben Jonson's English Grammar, published posthumously in 1640, records that /r/ was "sounded firme in the beginning of words, and more liquid in the middle, and ends."[8] Little more is said regarding /r/ until 1740, when one Mather Flint, writing in a primer for French learners of English, said: "...dans plusieurs mots, l’r devant une consonne est fort adouci, presque muet & rend un peu longue la voyale qui le precede." ("...in many words r before a consonant is greatly softened, almost mute, and slightly lengthens the preceding vowel.")[8] By the 1770s, postvocalic /r/-less pronunciation was becoming common around London even in more formal, educated speech. The English actor and linguist John Walker uses the spelling ar to indicate the long vowel of aunt in his 1775 rhyming dictionary.[2] In his influential Critical Pronouncing Dictionary and Expositor of the English Language (1791), Walker reported, with a strong tone of disapproval, that "...the r in lard, bard, [...] is pronounced so much in the throat as to be little more than the middle or Italian a, lengthened into baa, baad...."[8] Americans returning to England after the end of the American Revolutionary War in 1783 reported surprise at the significant changes in fashionable pronunciation.[9] By the early 19th century, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though it continued to be variable as late as the 1870s.[8]
The adoption of postvocalic /r/-less pronunciation as the British prestige standard in the late 18th and early 19th centuries influenced American port cities with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern port cities such as New York City, Boston, Alexandria, Charleston, and Savannah to become non-rhotic.[10] Like regional dialects in England, the accents of other areas in America remained rhotic in a display of linguistic "lag" that preserved the original pronunciation of /r/.[10] Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American prestige speech until the 1860s, when the American Civil War shifted America's centers of wealth and political power to areas with fewer cultural connections to the British elite.[11] This largely removed the prestige associated with non-rhotic pronunciation in America, such that when the advent of radio and television in the early 20th century established a national standard of American pronunciation, it became a rhotic variety that fully preserves historical /r/.[11]
Modern pronunciation
In most non-rhotic accents, if a word ending in written "r" is followed closely by a word beginning with a vowel, the /r/ is pronounced—as in water ice. This phenomenon is referred to as "linking R". Many non-rhotic speakers also insert an epenthetic /r/ between vowels when the first vowel is one that can occur before syllable-final r (drawring for drawing). This so-called "intrusive R" has been stigmatized, but nowadays many speakers of Received Pronunciation (RP) frequently "intrude" an epenthetic /r/ at word boundaries, especially where one or both vowels is schwa; for example the idea of it becomes the idea-r-of it, Australia and New Zealand becomes Australia-r-and New Zealand, the formerly well-known India-r-Office and "Laura Norder" (Law and Order). The typical alternative used by RP speakers (and some rhotic speakers as well) is to insert a glottal stop where an intrusive R would otherwise be placed.[12][13]
For non-rhotic speakers, what was historically a vowel plus /r/ is now usually realized as a long vowel. This is called compensatory lengthening, lengthening that occurs after the elision of a sound. So in RP and many other non-rhotic accents card, fern, born are pronounced [kʰɑːd], [fɜːn], [bɔːn] or similar (actual pronunciations vary from accent to accent). This length may be retained in phrases, so while car pronounced in isolation is [kʰɑː], car owner is [ˈkʰɑːɹəʊnə]. But a final schwa usually remains short, so water in isolation is [wɔːtʰə]. In RP and similar accents the vowels /iː/ and /uː/ (or /ʊ/), when followed by r, become diphthongs ending in schwa, so near is [nɪə] and poor is [pʰʊə], though these have other realizations as well, including monophthongal ones; once again, the pronunciations vary from accent to accent. The same happens to diphthongs followed by R, though these may be considered to end in /ər/ in rhotic speech, and it is the /ər/ that reduces to schwa as usual in non-rhotic speech: tire said in isolation is [tʰaɪə] and sour is [saʊə].[14] For some speakers, some long vowels alternate with a diphthong ending in schwa, so wear may be [wɛə] but wearing [wɛːɹɪŋ].
Even General American speakers commonly drop the /r/ in non-final unstressed syllables when another syllable in the same word also contains /r/; this may be referred to as R-dissimilation. Examples include the dropping of the first /r/ in the words surprise, governor and caterpillar. In more careful speech, however, the /r/ sounds are all retained.[15]
Distribution
Rhotic accents include Scottish English, Irish or Hiberno-English, most varieties of North American English, Barbadian English, Indian English,[17] and Pakistani English.[18]
Non-rhotic accents include most England's English, Welsh English, New Zealand English, Australian English, and South African English.
Semi-rhotic accents have also been studied, such as Jamaican English, in which r is pronounced (as in even non-rhotic accents) before vowels, but also in stressed monosyllables or stressed syllables at the ends of words (e.g. in "car" or "dare"); however, it is not pronounced at the end of unstressed syllables (e.g. in "water") or before consonants (e.g. "market").[19]
Variably rhotic accents, in which speakers often sporadically waver between rhoticity and non-rhoticity without any particular rules of context, are also widely documented. Variably rhotic accents comprise much of Caribbean English, for example, as spoken in Tobago, Guyana, Antigua and Barbuda, and the Bahamas.[20] They also include current-day New York City English,[21] New York Latino English, and some Boston English.
Non-rhotic accents in the Americas include the rest of Caribbean and Belize.
England
Though most English varieties in England are non-rhotic today, stemming from a trend towards this in southeastern England accelerating in the very late 1700s onwards, rhotic accents are still found in the West Country (south and west of a line from near Shrewsbury to around Portsmouth), the Corby area, some of Lancashire (north and west of the centre of Manchester), some parts of Yorkshire and Lincolnshire, and in the areas that border Scotland. The prestige form, however, exerts a steady pressure towards non-rhoticity. Thus the urban speech of Bristol or Southampton is more accurately described as variably rhotic, the degree of rhoticity being reduced as one moves up the class and formality scales.[22]
United States
American English is predominantly rhotic today, but at the end of the 1800s non-rhotic accents were common throughout much of the Eastern U.S. and through much of the South along the Gulf Coast. In fact, non-rhotic accents were established in all major U.S. cities along the Atlantic coast except for the Delaware Valley area around Philadelphia and Baltimore. This trend reversed during the early to mid-1900s. Non-rhotic accents have increasingly been perceived by Americans as sounding foreign or less educated with rhotic accents increasingly seen as sounding more "General American".[23]
Today, non-rhoticity in the American South is found primarily among older speakers, and only in some areas such as central and southern Alabama; Savannah, Georgia; and Norfolk, Virginia,[24] as well as in the Yat accent of New Orleans. The local dialects of eastern New England, especially Boston, Massachusetts, extending into the states of Maine and New Hampshire, are largely non-rhotic, as well as the traditional Rhode Island dialect; however, this feature has recently been receding. The New York City dialect is traditionally non-rhotic, though William Labov more precisely classifies its current form as variably rhotic,[25] with many of its sub-varieties now fully rhotic, such as in northeastern New Jersey.
African-American Vernacular English (AAVE for short) is largely non-rhotic, and in some non-rhotic Southern and AAVE accents, there is no linking r, that is, /r/ at the end of a word is deleted even when the following word starts with a vowel, so that "Mister Adams" is pronounced [mɪstə(ʔ)ˈædəmz].[26] In a few such accents, intervocalic /r/ is deleted before an unstressed syllable even within a word when the following syllable begins with a vowel. In such accents, pronunciations like [kæəˈlaːnə] for Carolina, or [bɛːˈʌp] for "bear up" are heard.[27] This pronunciation also occurs in AAVE.[28] This also occurred for many older non-rhotic Southern speakers.[29]
Typically, even non-rhotic modern American English varieties do pronounce the /r/ in /ɜːr/ (as in "bird," "work," or "perky"), realizing it, as in most of the U.S., as [ɝ] or [ɚ] listen.
Canada
Canadian English is entirely rhotic except for small isolated areas in southwestern New Brunswick, parts of Newfoundland, and the Lunenburg English variety spoken in Lunenburg and Shelburne Counties, Nova Scotia, which may be non-rhotic or variably rhotic.[30]
New Zealand
Although New Zealand English is predominantly non-rhotic, Southland and parts of Otago in the far south of New Zealand's South Island are rhotic from apparent Scottish influence. Some Māori speakers are semi-rhotic although it is not clearly identified to any particular region or attributed to any defined language shift. The Māori accent varies from the European-origin New Zealand accent.
Asia
The English spoken in Asia is predominantly rhotic. Many varieties of Indian English are rhotic owing to the underlying phonotactics of the native Indo-Aryan and Dravidian languages[17] whilst some tend to be non-rhotic. In the case of the Philippines, this may be explained because the English that is spoken there is heavily influenced by the American dialect. In addition, many East Asians (in China, Japan, Korea, and Taiwan) who have a good command of English generally have rhotic accents because of the influence of American English. This excludes Hong Kong, whose RP English dialect is a result of its almost 150-year-history as a British Crown colony (later British dependent territory). The lack of consonant /r/ in Cantonese also contributes to the phenomenon (although rhoticity started to exist due to the handover in 1997 and influence by US and East Asian entertainment industry). However, many older (and younger) speakers among South and East Asians speak non-rhotic.
Other Asian regions with non-rhotic English are Malaysia, Singapore, and Brunei. A typical Malaysian's English would be almost totally non-rhotic due to the nonexistence of rhotic endings in both languages of influence, whereas a more educated Malaysian's English may be non-rhotic due to Standard Malaysian English being based on RP (Received Pronunciation).
A typical teenager's Southeast Asian English would be rhotic, mainly because of prominent influence by American English. Spoken English in Myanmar is non-rhotic, but there are a number of English speakers with a rhotic or partially rhotic pronunciation. Sri Lankan English may be rhotic.
Africa
The English spoken in Africa (excluding South Africa) is based on British RP and is officially non-rhotic. Pronunciation and variation in African English accents are largely affected by native African language influences, level of education and exposure to western influences. The English accents spoken in the coastal areas of West Africa is primarily non-rhotic as are the underlying varieties of Niger-Congo languages spoken in that part of West Africa. Rhoticity may be present in English spoken in areas where rhotic Afro-Asiatic or Nilo Saharan languages are spoken across northern West Africa and in the Nilotic regions of East Africa. More modern trends show an increasing African-American influence on African English pronunciation particularly among younger urban affluent populations, where the American rhotic 'r' may be over-stressed in informal communication to create a pseudo-Americanised accent. By and large official spoken English used in post colonial African countries is non-rhotic. Standard Liberian English is also non-rhotic because liquids are lost at the end of words or before consonants.[31]
Mergers characteristic of non-rhotic accents
Some phonemic mergers are characteristic of non-rhotic accents. These usually include one item that historically contained an R (lost in the non-rhotic accent), and one that never did so. The section below lists mergers in order of approximately decreasing prevalence.
Panda–pander merger
In the terminology of John C. Wells, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets commA and lettER. It is found in all or nearly all non-rhotic accents,[32] and is even present in some accents that are in other respects rhotic, such as those of some speakers in Jamaica and the Bahamas.[32]
/ə/ | /ər/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
Ana | honor | ˈɑːnə | With father-bother merger. |
area | airier | ˈɛəriə | |
Basia | basher | ˈbæʃə | Without trap-bath split. |
Carla | collar | ˈkɑlə | With god-guard merger. |
Carta | Carter | ˈkɑːtə | |
cheetah | cheater | ˈtʃiːtə | |
Darla | dollar | ˈdɑlə | With god-guard merger. |
Dinah | diner | ˈdaɪnə | |
coca | coker | ˈkoʊkə | |
coda | coder | ˈkoʊdə | |
cola | coaler | ˈkoʊlə | |
coma | comber | ˈkoʊmə | |
custody | custardy | ˈkʌstədi | |
data | dater | ˈdeɪtə | |
Easton | eastern | ˈiːstən | |
Helena | Eleanor | ˈɛlənə | With h-dropping. Outside North America. |
eta | eater | ˈiːtə | |
eyen | iron | ˈaɪən | |
feta | fetter | ˈfɛtə | |
formally | formerly | ˈfɔːməli | |
geta | getter | ˈɡɛtə | |
ion | iron | ˈaɪən | |
karma | calmer | ˈkɑːmə | |
kava | carver | ˈkɑːvə | |
Lena | leaner | ˈliːnə | |
Lima | lemur | ˈliːmə | |
Lisa | leaser | ˈliːsə | |
Luna | lunar | ˈl(j)uːnə | |
Maia | Meier | ˈmaɪə | |
Maia | mire | ˈmaɪə | |
Maya | Meier | ˈmaɪə | |
Maya | mire | ˈmaɪə | |
manna | manner | ˈmænə | |
manna | manor | ˈmænə | |
Marta | martyr | ˈmɑːtə | |
Mia | mere | ˈmiə | |
miner | myna(h); mina(h) | ˈmaɪnə | |
minor | myna(h); mina(h) | ˈmaɪnə | |
Mona | moaner | ˈmoʊnə | |
Palma | palmer; Palmer | ˈpɑːmə | |
panda | pander | ˈpændə | |
parka | Parker | ˈpɑːkə | |
Parma | palmer; Palmer | ˈpɑːmə | |
Patton | pattern | ˈpætən | |
PETA | peter; Peter | ˈpiːtə | |
pharma | farmer | ˈfɑːmə | |
pita | peter; Peter | ˈpiːtə | |
Rhoda | rotor | ˈroʊɾə | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
Rita | reader | ˈriːɾə | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
Roma | roamer | ˈroʊmə | |
rota | rotor | ˈroʊtə | |
Saba | sabre; saber | ˈseɪbə | |
schema | schemer | ˈskiːmə | |
seven | Severn | ˈsɛvən | |
soya | sawyer | ˈsɔɪə | |
Stata | starter | ˈstɑːtə | With trap–bath split. |
taiga | tiger | ˈtaɪɡə | |
terra; Terra | terror | ˈtɛrə | |
tuba | tuber | ˈt(j)uːbə | |
tuna | tuner | ˈt(j)uːnə | |
Vespa | vesper | ˈvɛspə | |
via | veer | ˈvɪə | |
Wanda | wander | ˈwɒndə | |
Weston | western | ˈwɛstən | |
Wicca | wicker | ˈwɪkə |
Father–farther merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets PALM and START. It is found in the speech of the great majority of non-rhotic speakers, including those of England, Wales, the United States, the Caribbean, Australia, New Zealand and South Africa. It may be absent in some non-rhotic speakers in the Bahamas.[32]
/ɑː/ | /ɑːr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
ah | are | ˈɑː | |
ah | (h)our | ˈɑː | With smoothing. |
alms | arms | ˈɑːmz | |
alms | harms | ˈɑːmz | With H-dropping. |
balmy | barmy | ˈbɑːmi | |
calmer | karma | ˈkɑːmə | |
Chalmers | charmers | ˈtʃɑːməz | |
Dahmer | dharma | ˈdɑːmə | |
fa | far | ˈfɑː | |
father | farther | ˈfɑːðə | |
Jahn | yarn | ˈjɑːn | |
kava | carver | ˈkɑːvə | |
lava | larva | ˈlɑːvə | |
ma | mar | ˈmɑː | |
pa | par | ˈpɑː | |
Palma | Parma | ˈpɑːmə | |
palmer; Palmer | Parma | ˈpɑːmə | |
ska | scar | ˈskɑː | |
spa | spar | ˈspɑː | |
Stata | starter | ˈstɑːtə |
Pawn–porn merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and NORTH. It is found in most of the same accents as the father–farther merger described above, but is absent from the Bahamas and Guyana.[32]
/ɔː/ | /ɔːr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
alk | orc | ˈɔːk | |
auk | orc | ˈɔːk | |
aw | or | ˈɔː | |
awe | or | ˈɔː | |
awk | orc | ˈɔːk | |
balk | bork | ˈbɔːk | |
bawn | born | ˈbɔːn | |
caulk | cork | ˈkɔːk | |
cawed | chord | ˈkɔːd | |
cawed | cord | ˈkɔːd | |
draw | drawer | ˈdrɔː | |
gnaw | nor | ˈnɔː | |
hawk | orc | ˈɔːk | With H-dropping. |
laud | lord | ˈlɔːd | |
lawed | lord | ˈlɔːd | |
lawn | lorn | ˈlɔːn | |
pawn | porn | ˈpɔːn | |
sought | sort | ˈsɔːt | |
stalk | stork | ˈstɔːk | |
talk | torque | ˈtɔːk | |
taught | tort | ˈtɔːt | |
taut | tort | ˈtɔːt | |
taw | tor | ˈtɔː | |
thaw | Thor | ˈθɔː |
Caught–court merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and FORCE. It is found in those non-rhotic accents containing the pawn–porn merger that have also undergone the horse–hoarse merger. These include the accents of Southern England, Wales, non-rhotic New York City speakers, Trinidad and the Southern hemisphere. In such accents a three-way merger awe-or-ore/oar results. However, Labov et al. suggest that, in New York City English, this merger is present in perception not production. As in, although even locals perceive themselves using the same vowel in both cases, they tend to produce the force higher and more retracted than the vowel of thought.[33]
/ɔː/ | /ɔər/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
aw | oar | ˈɔː | |
aw | ore | ˈɔː | |
awe | oar | ˈɔː | |
awe | ore | ˈɔː | |
baud | board | ˈbɔːd | |
baud | bored | ˈbɔːd | |
bawd | board | ˈbɔːd | |
bawd | bored | ˈbɔːd | |
bawn | borne | ˈbɔːn | |
bawn | bourn; bourne; Bourne | ˈbɔːn | |
caught | court | ˈkɔːt | |
caw | core | ˈkɔː | |
daw | door | ˈdɔː | |
draw | drawer | ˈdrɔː | |
flaw | floor | ˈflɔː | |
fought | fort | ˈfɔːt | |
gaud | gored | ˈɡɔːd | |
haw | whore | ˈhɔː | |
law | lore | ˈlɔː | |
maw | more | ˈmɔː | |
maw | Moore | ˈmɔː | |
paw | pore | ˈpɔː | |
paw | pour | ˈpɔː | |
raw | roar | ˈrɔː | |
sauce | source | ˈsɔːs | |
saw | soar | ˈsɔː | |
saw | sore | ˈsɔː | |
sawed | soared | ˈsɔːd | |
sawed | sword | ˈsɔːd | |
Sean | shorn | ˈʃɔːn | |
shaw | shore | ˈʃɔː | |
Shawn | shorn | ˈʃɔːn | |
taw | tore | ˈtɔː | |
yaw | yore | ˈjɔː | |
yaw | your | ˈjɔː |
Calve–carve merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets BATH and START. It is found in some non-rhotic accents with broad A in words like "bath". It is general in southern England (excluding rhotic speakers), Trinidad, the Bahamas, and the Southern hemisphere. It is a possibility for Welsh, Eastern New England, Jamaican, and Guyanese speakers.
/aː/ | /ɑːr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
aunt | aren't | ˈɑːnt | |
calve | carve | ˈkɑːv | |
cast | karst | ˈkɑːst | |
caste | karst | ˈkɑːst | |
fast | farced | ˈfɑːst | |
Paw–poor merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and CURE. It is found in those non-rhotic accents containing the caught–court merger that have also undergone the pour–poor merger. Wells lists it unequivocally only for the accent of Trinidad, but it is an option for non-rhotic speakers in England, Australia and New Zealand. Such speakers have a potential four–way merger taw-tor-tore-tour.[34]
/ɔː/ | /ʊər/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
gaud | gourd | ˈɡɔːd | |
haw | whore | ˈhɔː | |
law | lure | ˈlɔː | With yod-dropping. |
maw | moor | ˈmɔː | |
maw | Moore | ˈmɔː | |
paw | poor | ˈpɔː | |
shaw | sure | ˈʃɔː | |
taw | tour | ˈtɔː | |
tawny | tourney | ˈtɔːni | |
yaw | your | ˈjɔː | |
yaw | you're | ˈjɔː |
Batted–battered merger
This merger is present in non-rhotic accents which have undergone the weak-vowel merger. Such accents include Australian, New Zealand, most South African speech, and some non-rhotic English speech (e.g. Norfolk, Sheffield).
A large number of homophonous pairs involve the syllabic -es and agentive -ers suffixes, such as merges-mergers and bleaches-bleachers. Because there are so many, they are excluded from the list of homophonous pairs below.
/ɪ̈/ | /ər/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
batted | battered | ˈbætəd | |
betted | bettered | ˈbɛtəd | |
busted | bustard | ˈbʌstəd | |
butches | butchers | ˈbʊtʃəz | |
butted | buttered | ˈbʌtəd | |
charted | chartered | ˈtʃɑ:təd | |
chatted | chattered | ˈtʃætəd | |
founded | foundered | ˈfaʊndəd | |
humid | humo(u)red | ˈhjuːməd | |
masted | mastered | ˈmæstəd, ˈmɑːstəd | |
matted | mattered | ˈmætəd | |
modding | modern | ˈmɒdən | With G-dropping. |
patted | pattered | ˈpætəd | |
patting | pattern | ˈpætən | With G-dropping. |
satin | Saturn | ˈsætən | |
scatted | scattered | ˈskætəd | |
splendid | splendo(u)red | ˈsplɛndəd | |
tatted | tattered | ˈtætəd | |
tended | tendered | ˈtɛndəd | |
territory | terror tree | ˈtɛrətriː | With happy-tensing. |
Dough–door merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets GOAT and FORCE. It may be found in some southern U.S. non-rhotic speech, some speakers of African American Vernacular English, some speakers in Guyana and some Welsh speech.[32]
/oʊ/ | /oʊr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
beau | boar | ˈboʊ | |
beau | bore | ˈboʊ | |
bode | board | ˈboʊd | |
bode | bored | ˈboʊd | |
bone | borne | ˈboʊn | |
bone | Bourne | ˈboʊn | |
bow | boar | ˈboʊ | |
bow | bore | ˈboʊ | |
bowed | board | ˈboʊd | |
bowed | bored | ˈboʊd | |
chose | chores | ˈtʃoʊz | |
coast | coursed | ˈkoʊst | |
coat | court | ˈkoʊt | |
code | cored | ˈkoʊd | |
doe | door | ˈdoʊ | |
does | doors | ˈdoʊz | |
dough | door | ˈdoʊ | |
doze | doors | ˈdoʊz | |
floe | floor | ˈfloʊ | |
flow | floor | ˈfloʊ | |
foe | fore | ˈfoʊ | |
foe | four | ˈfoʊ | |
go | gore | ˈɡoʊ | |
goad | gored | ˈɡoʊd | |
hoe | whore | ˈhoʊ | |
hoed | hoard | ˈhoʊd | |
hoed | horde | ˈhoʊd | |
hoed | whored | ˈhoʊd | |
hose | whores | ˈhoʊz | |
lo | lore | ˈloʊ | |
low | lore | ˈloʊ | |
moan | mourn | ˈmoʊn | |
Moe | Moore | ˈmoʊ | |
Moe | more | ˈmoʊ | |
Mona | mourner | ˈmoʊnə | |
mow | Moore | ˈmoʊ | |
mow | more | ˈmoʊ | |
mown | mourn | ˈmoʊn | |
O | oar | ˈoʊ | |
O | ore | ˈoʊ | |
ode | oared | ˈoʊd | |
oh | oar | ˈoʊ | |
oh | ore | ˈoʊ | |
owe | oar | ˈoʊ | |
owe | ore | ˈoʊ | |
owed | oared | ˈoʊd | |
Po | pore | ˈpoʊ | |
Po | pour | ˈpoʊ | |
Poe | pore | ˈpoʊ | |
Poe | pour | ˈpoʊ | |
poach | porch | ˈpoʊtʃ | |
poke | pork | ˈpoʊk | |
pose | pores | ˈpoʊz | |
pose | pours | ˈpoʊz | |
road | roared | ˈroʊd | |
rode | roared | ˈroʊd | |
roe | roar | ˈroʊ | |
rose | roars | ˈroʊz | |
row | roar | ˈroʊ | |
rowed | roared | ˈroʊd | |
sew | soar | ˈsoʊ | |
sew | sore | ˈsoʊ | |
sewed | soared | ˈsoʊd | |
sewed | sored | ˈsoʊd | |
sewed | sword | ˈsoʊd | |
shone | shorn | ˈʃoʊn | |
show | shore | ˈʃoʊ | |
shown | shorn | ˈʃoʊn | |
snow | snore | ˈsnoʊ | |
so | soar | ˈsoʊ | |
so | sore | ˈsoʊ | |
sow | soar | ˈsoʊ | |
sow | sore | ˈsoʊ | |
sowed | soared | ˈsoʊd | |
sowed | sored | ˈsoʊd | |
sowed | sword | ˈsoʊd | |
stow | store | ˈstoʊ | |
Thoth | tort | ˈtoʊt | With th-stopping. |
toad | toward | ˈtoʊd | |
toe | tore | ˈtoʊ | |
toed | toward | ˈtoʊd | |
tone | torn | ˈtoʊn | |
tow | tore | ˈtoʊ | |
towed | toward | ˈtoʊd | |
woe | wore | ˈwoʊ | |
whoa | wore | ˈwoʊ | With wine–whine merger. |
yo | yore | ˈjoʊ | |
yo | your | ˈjoʊ |
Show–sure merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets GOAT and CURE. It may be present in those speakers who have both the dough–door merger described above, and also the pour–poor merger. These include some southern U.S. non-rhotic speakers, some speakers of African American Vernacular English and some speakers in Guyana.[32] It can be seen in the term "Fo Sho", an imitation of "for sure".
/oʊ/ | /ʊːr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
beau | Boer | ˈboʊ | |
beau | boor | ˈboʊ | |
bow | Boer | ˈboʊ | |
bow | boor | ˈboʊ | |
goad | gourd | ˈɡoʊd | |
hoe | whore | ˈhoʊ | |
lo | lure | ˈloʊ | With yod-dropping. |
low | lure | ˈloʊ | With yod-dropping. |
Moe | moor | ˈmoʊ | |
Moe | Moore | ˈmoʊ | |
mode | moored | ˈmoʊd | |
mow | moor | ˈmoʊ | |
mow | Moore | ˈmoʊ | |
mowed | moored | ˈmoʊd | |
Po | poor | ˈpoʊ | |
Poe | poor | ˈpoʊ | |
shew | sure | ˈʃoʊ | |
show | sure | ˈʃoʊ | |
toad | toured | ˈtoʊd | |
toe | tour | ˈtoʊ | |
toed | toured | ˈtoʊd | |
tow | tour | ˈtoʊ | |
towed | toured | ˈtoʊd | |
yo | your | ˈjoʊ | |
yo | you're | ˈjoʊ |
Often–orphan merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets CLOTH and NORTH. It may be present in old-fashioned Eastern New England accents,[35] New York City speakers[36] and also in some speakers in Jamaica and Guyana. The merger was also until recently present in the dialects of southern England, including Received Pronunciation—specifically, the phonemic merger of the words often and orphan was a running gag in the Gilbert and Sullivan musical, The Pirates of Penzance.
/ɔː/ | /ɔːr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
boss | bourse | ˈbɔːs | |
hoss[37] | horse | ˈhɔːs | |
moss | Morse | ˈmɔːs | |
off | Orff; orfe; orf | ˈɔːf | |
often | orphan | ˈɔːfən | "Often" is pronounced with a sounded T by some speakers. |
God–guard merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets LOT and START. It may be present in non-rhotic accents that have undergone the father–bother merger. These may include some New York accents,[38] some southern U.S. accents,[39] and African American Vernacular English.[40]
/ɑ/ | /ɑr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
bob; Bob | barb; Barb | ˈbɑb | |
bock | bark | ˈbɑk | |
bocks | barks | ˈbɑks | |
bocks | Berks | ˈbɑks | |
bod | bard | ˈbɑd | |
bod | barred | ˈbɑd | |
boff | barf | ˈbɑf | |
bot | Bart | ˈbɑt | |
box | barks | ˈbɑks | |
box | Berks | ˈbɑks | |
clock | Clark; Clarke | ˈklɑk | |
clock | clerk | ˈklɑk | |
cob | carb | ˈkɑb | |
cod | card | ˈkɑd | |
collar | Carla | ˈkɑlə | |
collie | Carlie | ˈkɑli | |
cop | carp | ˈkɑp | |
cot | cart | ˈkɑt | |
dock | dark | ˈdɑk | |
dollar | Darla | ˈdɑlə | |
dolling | darling | ˈdɑlɪŋ | |
don; Don | darn | ˈdɑn | |
dot | dart | ˈdɑt | |
god | garred | ˈɡɑd | |
god | guard | ˈɡɑd | |
hock | hark | ˈhɑk | |
holly; Holly | Harley | ˈhɑli | |
hominy | harmony | ˈhɑməni | With weak vowel merger. |
hop | harp | ˈhɑp | |
hot | hart | ˈhɑt | |
hot | heart | ˈhɑt | |
hottie | hardy | ˈhɑɾi | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
hottie | hearty | ˈhɑti | |
hough | hark | ˈhɑk | |
hovered | Harvard | ˈhɑvəd | |
knock | narc | ˈnɑk | |
knock | nark | ˈnɑk | |
knocks | narcs | ˈnɑks | |
knocks | narks | ˈnɑks | |
Knox | narcs | ˈnɑk | |
Knox | narks | ˈnɑk | |
lock | lark | ˈlɑk | |
Locke | lark | ˈlɑk | |
lodge | large | ˈlɑdʒ | |
lop | larp | ˈlɑp | |
mock | mark; Mark | ˈmɑk | |
mocks | marks; Mark's | ˈmɑks | |
mocks | Marx | ˈmɑks | |
mod | marred | ˈmɑd | |
modge | Marge | ˈmɑdʒ | |
moll; Moll | marl | ˈmɑl | |
molly; Molly | Marley | ˈmɑli | |
mosh | marsh | ˈmɑʃ | |
nock | narc | ˈnɑk | |
nock | nark | ˈnɑk | |
nocks | narcs | ˈnɑks | |
nocks | narks | ˈnɑks | |
Nox | narcs | ˈnɑk | |
Nox | narks | ˈnɑk | |
ox | arcs | ˈɑks | |
ox | arks | ˈɑks | |
pock | park; Park | ˈpɑk | |
pocks | parks; Park's | ˈpɑks | |
polly; Polly | parley; Parley | ˈpɑli | |
pot | part | ˈpɑt | |
potch | parch | ˈpɑtʃ | |
potty | party | ˈpɑti | |
pox | parks | ˈpɑks | |
shod | shard | ˈʃɑd | |
shock | shark | ˈʃɑk | |
shop | sharp | ˈʃɑp | |
shopping | sharpen | ˈʃɑpən | With weak vowel merger and G-dropping. |
sock | Sark | ˈsɑk | |
sod | Sard | ˈsɑd | |
Spock | spark | ˈspɑk | |
spotter | Sparta | ˈspɑtə | |
stock | stark | ˈstɑk | |
tod | tard | ˈtɑd | |
tod | tarred | ˈtɑd | |
Todd | tard | ˈtɑd | |
Todd | tarred | ˈtɑd | |
top | tarp | ˈtɑp | |
tot | tart | ˈtɑt | |
yon | yarn | ˈjɑn |
Shot–short merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets LOT and NORTH. It may be present in some Eastern New England accents.[41][42]
/ɒ/ | /ɒr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
bon | born | ˈbɒːn | |
box | borks | ˈbɒːks | |
cock | cork; Cork | ˈkɒːk | |
cocks | corks; Cork's | ˈkɒːks | |
cops | corpse | ˈkɒːps | |
cox | corks; Cork's | ˈkɒːks | |
cod | chord | ˈkɒːd | |
cod | cord | ˈkɒːd | |
con | corn | ˈkɒːn | |
dock | dork | ˈdɒːk | |
fox | forks | ˈfɒːks | |
dom | dorm | ˈdɒːm | |
mog | morgue | ˈmɒːɡ | |
mot | Mort | ˈmɒːt | |
odder | order | ˈɒːdə | |
otter | order | ˈɒːɾə | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
ox | orcs | ˈɒːks | |
pond | porned | ˈpɒːnd | |
pock | pork | ˈpɒːk | |
posh | Porsche | ˈpɒːʃ | |
pot | port | ˈpɒːt | |
scotch; Scotch | scorch | ˈskɒːtʃ | |
shoddy | shorty | ˈʃɒːɾi | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
shot | short | ˈʃɒːt | |
snot | snort | ˈsnɒːt | |
sob | Sorb | ˈsɒːb | |
solder | sorter | ˈsɒːɾə | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
sot | sort | ˈsɒːt | |
Spock | spork | ˈspɒːk | |
spot | sport | ˈspɒːt | |
stock | stork | ˈstɒːk | |
swan | sworn | ˈswɒːn | |
swat | swart | ˈswɒːt | |
tock | torque | ˈtɒːk | |
tot | tort | ˈtɒːt | |
tox | torques | ˈtɒːks | |
wabble | warble | ˈwɒːbəl | |
wad | ward | ˈwɒːd | |
wad | warred | ˈwɒːd | |
wan | warn | ˈwɒːn | |
wand | warned | ˈwɒːnd | |
watt | wart | ˈwɒːt | |
whap | warp | ˈwɒːp | With wine–whine merger. |
what | wart | ˈwɒːt | With wine–whine merger. |
whop | warp | ˈwɒːp | With wine–whine merger. |
wobble | warble | ˈwɒːbəl | |
yock | York | ˈjɒːk |
Bud–bird merger
A merger of /ɜː(r)/ and /ʌ/ occurring for some speakers of Jamaican English making bud and bird homophones as /bʌd/.[43] The conversion of /ɜː/ to [ʌ] or [ə] is also found in places scattered around England and Scotland. Some speakers, mostly rural, in the area from London to Norfolk exhibit this conversion, mainly before voiceless fricatives. This gives pronunciation like first [fʌst] and worse [wʌs]. The word cuss appears to derive from the application of this sound change to the word curse. Similarly, lurve is coined from love.
/ʌ/ | /ɜːr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
blood | blurred | ˈblʌd | |
bub | burb | ˈbʌb | |
buck | Burke | ˈbʌk | |
Buckley | Berkeley | ˈbʌkli | |
bud | bird | ˈbʌd | |
bud | burred | ˈbʌd | |
budging | burgeon | ˈbʌdʒən | With weak vowel merger and G-dropping. |
bug | berg | ˈbʌɡ | |
bug | burg | ˈbʌɡ | |
bugger | burger | ˈbʌɡə | |
bugging | bergen; Bergen | ˈbʌɡən | With weak vowel merger and G-dropping. |
bummer | Burma | ˈbʌmə | |
bun | Bern | ˈbʌn | |
bun | burn | ˈbʌn | |
bunt | burnt | ˈbʌnt | |
bused; bussed | burst | ˈbʌst | |
bust | burst | ˈbʌst | |
but | Bert | ˈbʌt | |
but | Burt | ˈbʌt | |
butt | Bert | ˈbʌt | |
butt | Burt | ˈbʌt | |
button | Burton | ˈbʌtən | |
buzz | burrs | ˈbʌz | |
chuck | chirk | ˈtʃʌk | |
cluck | clerk | ˈklʌk | |
colo(u)r | curler | ˈkʌlə | |
coven | curving | ˈkʌvən | With weak vowel merger and G-dropping. |
cub | curb | ˈkʌb | |
cub | kerb | ˈkʌb | |
cud | curd | ˈkʌd | |
cud | curred | ˈkʌd | |
cud | Kurd | ˈkʌd | |
cuddle | curdle | ˈkʌdəl | |
cuff you | curfew | ˈkʌfju | |
cull | curl | ˈkʌl | |
culler | curler | ˈkʌlə | |
cunning | kerning | ˈkʌnɪŋ | |
cuss | curse | ˈkʌs | |
cut | curt; Curt | ˈkʌt | |
cutting | curtain | ˈkʌtɪn | With G-dropping. |
dost | durst | ˈdʌst | |
doth | dearth | ˈdʌθ | |
duck | dirk | ˈdʌk | |
ducked | dirked | ˈdʌkt | |
ducks | dirks | ˈdʌks | |
duct | dirked | ˈdʌkt | |
dust | durst | ˈdʌst | |
dux | dirks | ˈdʌks | |
fud | furred | ˈfʌd | |
fun | fern | ˈfʌn | |
fussed | first | ˈfʌst | |
fuzz | furs | ˈfʌz | |
gull | girl | ˈɡʌl | |
gully | girly | ˈɡʌli | |
gutter | girder | ˈɡʌɾə | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
hub | herb | ˈ(h)ʌb | With or without H-dropping. |
huck | Herc | ˈhʌk | |
huck | irk | ˈʌk | With H-dropping. |
huddle | hurdle | ˈhʌdəl | |
hull | hurl | ˈhʌl | |
hum | herm | ˈhʌm | |
Hun | earn | ˈʌn | With H-dropping. |
Hun | urn | ˈʌn | With H-dropping. |
hush | Hirsch | ˈhʌʃ | |
hut | hurt | ˈhʌt | |
love | lurve | ˈlʌv | |
luck | lurk | ˈlʌk | |
lucks | lurks | ˈlʌks | |
lunt | learnt | ˈlʌnt | |
luxe | lurks | ˈlʌks | |
much | merch | ˈmʌtʃ | |
muck | merc | ˈmʌk | |
muck | mirk | ˈmʌk | |
muck | murk | ˈmʌk | |
muddle | myrtle | ˈmʌɾəl | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
mudder | murder | ˈmʌdə | |
mull | merl | ˈmʌl | |
mutter | murder | ˈmʌɾə | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
mutton | Merton | ˈmʌtən | |
oven | Irving | ˈʌvən | With weak vowel merger and G-dropping. |
puck | perk | ˈpʌk | |
pudge | purge | ˈpʌdʒ | |
pup | perp | ˈpʌp | |
pus | purse | ˈpʌs | |
pussy (pus) | Percy | ˈpʌsi | |
putt | pert | ˈpʌt | |
scut | skirt | ˈskʌt | |
shuck | shirk | ˈʃʌk | |
spun | spurn | ˈspʌn | |
stud | stirred | ˈstʌd | |
such | search | ˈsʌtʃ | |
suck | cirque | ˈsʌk | |
suckle | circle | ˈsʌkəl | |
suffer | surfer | ˈsʌfə | |
sully | surly | ˈsʌli | |
Sutton | certain | ˈsʌtən | With weak vowel merger. |
thud | third | ˈθʌd | |
ton(ne) | tern | ˈtʌn | |
ton(ne) | turn | ˈtʌn | |
tough | turf | ˈtʌf | |
tuck | Turk | ˈtʌk | |
tucks | Turks | ˈtʌks | |
Tuttle | turtle | ˈtʌtəl | |
tux | Turks | ˈtʌks | |
us | Erse | ˈʌs | |
wont | weren't | ˈwʌnt |
Oil–earl merger / coil–curl merger
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets CHOICE and NURSE preconsonantally (so that "stir" and "boy" never rhymed). It was present in older New York and New Orleans regional accents, but became stigmatized and is sharply recessive in those born since the Second World War.[44] This merger is known for the word soitanly, used often by the Three Stooges comedian Curly Howard as a variant of certainly in comedy shorts of the 1930s and 1940s.
/ɔɪ/ | /ɜr/ | IPA | Notes |
---|---|---|---|
adjoin | adjourn | əˈdʒɜɪn | |
boil | burl | ˈbɜɪl | |
Boyd | bird | ˈbɜɪd | |
Boyle | burl | ˈbɜɪl | |
Boyd | bird | ˈbɜɪd | |
coil | curl | ˈkɜɪl | |
coin | kern | ˈkɜɪn | |
coitus | Curtis | ˈkɜɪtəs | With weak vowel merger. |
foil | furl | ˈfɜɪl | |
goitre; goiter | girder | ˈɡɜɪɾər | With intervocalic alveolar flapping. |
hoist | Hearst | ˈhɜɪst | |
hoist | hurst; Hurst | ˈhɜɪst | |
Hoyle | hurl | ˈhɜɪl | |
loin | learn | ˈlɜɪn | |
oil | earl | ˈɜɪl | |
poil | pearl | ˈpɜɪl | |
poise | purrs | ˈpɜɪz | |
toyed | turd | ˈtɜɪd | |
voice | verse | ˈvɜɪs | |
Voight | vert | ˈvɜɪt |
Other mergers
In some accents, syllabification may interact with rhoticity, resulting in homophones where non-rhotic accents have centering diphthongs. Possibilities include Korea–career,[45] Shi'a–sheer, and Maia–mire,[46] while skua may be identical with the second syllable of obscure.[47]
Effect of non-rhotic dialects on orthography
Certain words have spellings derived from non-rhotic dialects or renderings of foreign words through non-rhotic pronunciation. Examples include:
- Er, used in non-rhotic dialects to indicate a filled pause, which most rhotic dialects would instead convey with uh or eh.
- The Korean family name 박 (Bak/Pak) usually written "Park" in English.
- The game Parcheesi.
- British English slang words:
- char for cha from the Mandarin Chinese pronunciation of 茶 (= "tea" (the drink))
- In Rudyard Kipling's books:
- Burma and Myanmar for Burmese [bəmà] and [mjàmmà].
- Transliteration of Cantonese words and names, such as char siu (Chinese: 叉燒; Jyutping: caa¹ siu¹) and Wong Kar-wai (Chinese: 王家衛; Jyutping: Wong⁴ Gaa1wai⁶)
- The spelling of schoolmarm for school ma'am.
- The slang word nark for "informant", from Romany language nāk "nose".
See also
References
Notes
Citations
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Lass (1999), p. 114.
- 1 2 3 Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006): 47.
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 216.
- ↑ Paul Skandera, Peter Burleigh, A Manual of English Phonetics and Phonology, Gunter Narr Verlag, 2011, p. 60.
- ↑ Based on H. Orton, et al., Survey of English Dialects (1962–71). Some areas with partial rhoticity, such as parts of the East Riding of Yorkshire, are not shaded on this map.
- ↑ Based on P. Trudgill, The Dialects of England.
- ↑ Lass (1999), pp. 114–15.
- 1 2 3 4 Lass (1999), p. 115.
- ↑ Fisher (2001), p. 73.
- 1 2 Fisher (2001), p. 76.
- 1 2 Fisher (2001), p. 77.
- ↑ Wells, Accents of English, 1:224-225.
- ↑ Gimson, Alfred Charles (2014), Cruttenden, Alan, ed., Gimson's Pronunciation of English (8th ed.), Routledge, pp. 119–120, ISBN 978-1-4441-8309-2
- ↑ Shorter Oxford English Dictionary
- ↑ Wells, Accents of English, p. 490.
- ↑ Wakelyn, Martin: "Rural dialects in England", in: Trudgill, Peter (1984): Language in the British Isles, p.77
- 1 2 Wells, J. C. (1982). Accents of English 3: Beyond the British Isles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. p. 629. ISBN 0-521-28541-0.
- ↑
- ↑ Wells, Accents of English, pp. 76, 221
- ↑ Schneider, Edgar (2008). Varieties of English: The Americas and the Caribbean. Walter de Gruyter. p. 396.
- ↑ McClear, Sheila (2 June 2010). "Why the classic Noo Yawk accent is fading away". New York Post. Retrieved 13 April 2013.
- ↑ Trudgill, Peter (1984). Language in the British Isles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-28409-7.
- ↑ Milla, Robert McColl (2012). English Historical Sociolinguistics. Edinburgh University Press. pp. 25–26. ISBN 978-0-7486-4181-9.
- ↑ Labov, Ash, and Boberg, 2006: pp. 47–48.
- ↑ Trudgill, Peter (2010). Investigations in Sociohistorical Linguistics. Cambridge University Press.
- ↑ Gick, Bryan. 1999. A gesture-based account of intrusive consonants in English. Phonology 16: 1, pp. 29–54. (pdf). Retrieved 12 November 2010.
- ↑ Harris 2006: pp. 2–5.
- ↑ Pollock et al., 1998.
- ↑ http://www.atlas.mouton-content.com/secure/generalmodules/varieties/unit0000/virtualsession/vslessons/thomas.pdf] p. 16
- ↑ Trudgill, Peter (2000). "Sociohistorical linguistics and dialect survival: a note on another Nova Scotian enclave". In Magnus Leung, ed. Language Structure and Variation. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell International. p. 197.
- ↑ Brinton, Lauren and Leslie Arnovick. The English Language: A Linguistic History. Oxford University Press: Canada, 2006
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 Wells (1982)
- ↑ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), p. 235
- ↑ Wells, p. 287
- ↑ Wells, p. 524
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 503
- ↑ Dialectal variant of "horse"
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 504
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 544
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 577
- ↑ Wells, p. 520
- ↑ Dillard, Joey Lee (1980). Perspectives on American English. The Hague; New York: Walter de Gruyter. p. 53. ISBN 90-279-3367-7.
- ↑ Wells, John C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-22919-7., pp. 136–37, 203–6, 234, 245–47, 339–40, 400, 419, 443, 576
- ↑ Wells (1982), pp. 508-509
- ↑ Wells (1982), p. 225
- ↑ Upton, Clive; Eben Upton (2004). Oxford rhyming dictionary. Oxford University Press. p. 59. ISBN 0-19-280115-5.
- ↑ Upton, Clive; Eben Upton (2004). Oxford rhyming dictionary. Oxford University Press. p. 60. ISBN 0-19-280115-5.
Works cited
- Fisher, John Hurt (2001). "British and American, Continuity and Divergence". In Algeo, John. The Cambridge History of the English Language, Volume VI: English in North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 59–85. ISBN 0-521-26479-0.
- Labov, William; Ash, Sharon; Boberg, Charles (2006). The Atlas of North American English. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3-11-016746-8.
- Lass, Roger (1999). "Phonology and Morphology". In Lass, Roger. The Cambridge History of the English Language, Volume III: 1476–1776. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 56–186. ISBN 0-521-26476-6.
- Pollock, Bailey, Berni, Fletcher, Hinton, Johnson, Roberts, & Weaver (17 March 2001). "Phonological Features of African American Vernacular English (AAVE)". Retrieved 8 November 2016.
- Trudgill, Peter (1984). Language in the British Isles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Wells, J. C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.