Behavioral enrichment

An Asian elephant in a zoo manipulating a suspended ball provided as environmental enrichment.

Behavioral enrichment (closely related to environmental enrichment) is an animal husbandry principle that seeks to enhance the quality of captive animal care by identifying and providing the environmental stimuli necessary for optimal psychological and physiological well-being.[1] The goal of environmental enrichment is to improve or maintain an animal's physical and psychological health by increasing the range or number of species-specific behaviors, increasing positive utilization of the captive environment, preventing or reducing the frequency of abnormal behaviors such as stereotypies, and increasing the individual's ability to cope with the challenges of captivity. The purpose of behavioral enrichment is to improve the overall welfare of animals in captivity and create a habitat similar to what they would experience in their wild environment.[2]

For each animal in captivity, a goal oriented plan is outlined and created to meet the needs of that animal specifically.[2] Many different factors are included in making an enrichment outline including the needs of the species, their desired behaviors, an individual history, and the animal's current habitat. This plan is then put into action and changed based on the response and changing needs of the animal.[3]

A variety of enrichment techniques are used to create desired outcomes similar to an animals individual and species' history. Each of the techniques used are intended to stimulate the animal's senses similarly to how they would be activated in the wild. Provided enrichment may be seen in the form of auditory, olfactory, habitat factors, food, research projects, training, and objects.[4]

Environmental enrichment can be offered to any animal in captivity, including:

Environmental enrichment can be beneficial to a wide range of vertebrates and invertebrates such as land mammals, marine mammals, and amphibians.[9] In the United States, specific regulations must be followed for enrichment plans in order to guarantee, regulate, and provide appropriate living environments and stimulation for animals in captivity.[10]

Making a goal oriented plan

Creating an individual goal oriented plan for each animal in captivity is a crucial part of behavioral enrichment. Each plan is created by considering desired species specific behavior, their habitat while in captivity, and their individual history. The plan created for an animal often also includes planning in order to discourage unwanted and abnormal behaviors that are acquired from being in captivity.[2] Every plan that is created must have enrichment activities that can be well documented as a way to ensure that the animal is benefitting from the plan and activities they take part in. By creating a plan with easily documented activities, caretakers are able to adjust the plan as needed for desired behaviors of the individual animal.[3]

Each plan can be created following a set of steps. First, the animal must be identified and a team of specialized members is assembled. The team members often have specific roles, such as gathering information regarding the species, documenting and analyzing the environment, and going over the animal's individual history. The next step is to gather the information and create a plan with specific and detailed enrichment activities that will be used to help produce the desired behaviors that were agreed upon by team members. After the plan is drawn up, it is then submitted to the Animal Management team who then approves or denies the plan.[11] Once the plan is approved, the caretakers for the animal begin picking different activities off of the list of approved ideas and present them to the animal as a way to ensure varied enrichment. As enrichment activities begin, a schedule is then produced in order to document the animal's change and progression; it is at this point that the plan is reevaluated and adjusted depending on the animal's individual responses and new behaviors.[2]

Plans are created for each animal in captivity. This includes land mammals, amphibians, and even marine mammals. Dolphins, sea lions, whales, sharks, and even fish that are seen within captivity and throughout aquariums are all given goal oriented enrichment plans. Marine mammals experience training with food and positive feedback as a major form of their enrichment plans. This training helps zookeepers and trainers monitor the animal's health and veterinary care in a format that is less stressful and safer for the animal.[9]

Types of enrichment

Behavioral enrichment - Feeding
Behavioral enrichment - Feeding
Behavioral enrichment - Sensory
Behavioral enrichment - Sensory

Any stimulus which evokes an animal's interest in a positive way can be considered enriching, including natural and artificial objects, scents, novel foods, and different methods of preparing foods (for example, frozen in ice). Most enrichment stimuli can be divided into seven groups:[4]

Elaborate systems of food presentation have been developed (e.g. presenting dead rats for wildcats in a Swedish zoo) and computer programmed devices which allow the animals in the enclosure to search for prey as they would in their natural environment.

It can be argued that a stimulus may be considered enriching even if the animal's reaction to it is negative, such as with unpleasant scents, although stimuli that evoke extreme stress or fear should be avoided, as well as stimuli that can be harmful to the animal. A contrary point of view is that for environmental enrichment to be considered successful, it should promote only positive behaviours.

Enclosures in modern zoos are often designed to facilitate environmental enrichment. For example, the Denver Zoo's exhibit Predator Ridge allows different African carnivores to be rotated among several enclosures, providing the animals with a different sized environment and exposing them to each other's scents.

Assessing the success

A range of methods can be used to assess which environmental enrichments should be provided. These are based on the premises that captive animals should perform behaviours in a similar way to those in the ethogram of their ancestral species,[12] animals should be allowed to perform the activities or interactions they prefer, i.e. preference test studies,[13] and animals should be allowed to perform those activities for which they are highly motivated, i.e. motivation studies.[14]

Environmental enrichment is a way to ensure that an animals natural and instinctual behaviors are kept and able to be passed and taught from one generation to the next. Enrichment techniques that encourage species specific behaviors, like those that are discovered in the wild, have been studied and found to help the process of reintroduction of endangered species into their natural habitats, as well as helping to create offspring with natural traits and behaviors.[15]

The main way the success of environmental enrichment can be measured is by recognizing the behavioral changes that occur from the techniques used to shape desired behaviors of the animal compared to the behaviors of those found in the wild.[16] Other ways that the success of environmental enrichment can be assessed quantitatvely by a range of behavioral and physiological indicators of animal welfare. In addition to those listed above, behavioral indicators include the occurrence of abnormal behaviours (e.g. stereotypies,[17][18] cognitive bias studies,[19] and the effects of frustration.[20][21] Physiological indicators include heart rate,[22] corticosteroids,[23] immune function,[24] neuorobiology,[25] eggshell quality[26] and thermography.[27]

It is very difficult for zookeepers to measure the effectiveness of enrichment in terms of the stress due to the fact that animals that are found in zoos are oftentimes on display and presented with very abnormal conditions that can cause uneasiness and stress. Measuring enrichment in terms of reproduction is easier because of our ability to record offspring numbers and fertility. By making necessary environment changes and providing mental stimulation, animals in captivity have been seen to reproduce at a more similar rate to their wild ancestors in comparison to those provided with less behavioral and environmental enrichment.[16]

Regulatory requirements

In the United States, the 1985 amendments to the United States Animal Welfare Act amendments directed the Secretary of Agriculture to establish regulations to provide an adequate physical environment to promote the psychological well-being of primates[28] and exercise for dogs.[29] Subsequent standards for nonhuman primate environmental enhancement (including provisions for social grouping and environmental enrichment) are included under Section 3.81 in the Animal Welfare Regulations (9 CFR).[30] Concepts relating to behavioral needs and environmental enrichment are also incorporated into the standards for marine, flying, and aquatic mammals.[31]

The Association of Zoos and Aquariums (also known as the AZA), requires that animal husbandry and welfare be a main concern for those caring for animals in captivity. In order to be an AZA accredited facility, certain guidelines must be followed that pertain to the practices and techniques of enrichment in zoos. These regulations are in place in order to protect the animals overall wellbeing and to ensure the proper mental, physical, social, and biological characteristics of the animals are present when being compared to their ancestors that are found in the wild.[32]

When evaluating a facility for requirements and aspects necessary to receive accreditation as an AZA facility, all aspects are reviewed. This includes, but is not limited to, all animal habitats, diets, enrichment plans, and social groupings found at the location being reviewed. The facility is also evaluated on research done at the institution, their veterinary care in place for the animals, public education efforts, and overall involvement in wildlife conservation.[33]

References

  1. Shepherdson, D.J. (1998) “Tracing the path of environmental enrichment in zoos” in Shepherdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D. and Hutchins, M. (1998) Second Nature – Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals, 1st Edition, Smithsonian Institution Press, London, UK, pp. 1 – 12.
  2. 1 2 3 4 "Process | Cleveland Metroparks Zoo". www.clevelandmetroparks.com. Retrieved 2016-03-29.
  3. 1 2 "Enrichment". www.aza.org. 2010-12-10. Archived from the original on 2016-04-09. Retrieved 2016-03-29.
  4. 1 2 "Animal Enrichment - National Zoo". nationalzoo.si.edu. Retrieved 2016-03-29.
  5. Maple TL (2007). "Toward a science of welfare for animals in the zoo" (PDF). J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 10 (1): 63–70. PMID 17484680. doi:10.1080/10888700701277659.
  6. Ron Hines, D.V.M. (2006-04-24). "Synopsis of the Environmental Enrichment Program of 2nd Chance Sanctuary". Archived from the original on 10 June 2006. Retrieved 2006-06-11.
  7. Sherwin, C.M. (2007). "Validating refinements to laboratory housing: asking the animals.". Retrieved April 9, 2013.
  8. Hubrecht, R. (1995). Dogs and dog housing. In, Smith, C.P. and V. Taylor (Eds) Environmental Enrichment Information Resources for Laboratory Animals. Universities Federation for Animal Welfare (UFAW), Potters Bar, Herts. pp. 49-62
  9. 1 2 "Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums". www.ammpa.org. Retrieved 2016-03-29.
  10. Kulpa-Eddy, Jodie A.; Taylor, Sylvia; Adams, Kristina M. (2005-01-01). "USDA Perspective on Environmental Enrichment for Animals". ILAR Journal. 46 (2): 83–94. ISSN 1084-2020. PMID 15775018. doi:10.1093/ilar.46.2.83.
  11. "Animal Management". www.aza.org. 2013-02-27. Archived from the original on 2016-05-16. Retrieved 2016-03-29.
  12. Dawkins, M.S., (1989). Time budgets in red junglefowl as a baseline for the assessment of welfare in domestic-fowl. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 24: 77-80. doi:10.1016/0168-1591(89)90126-3
  13. Sherwin, C.M. and Glen, E.F., (2003). Cage colour preferences and effects of home-cage colour on anxiety in laboratory mice. Animal Behaviour, 66: 1085-1092
  14. Sherwin, C.M., (2004). The motivation of group-housed laboratory mice, Mus musculus, for additional space. Animal Behaviour, 67: 711-717. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.018
  15. Shepherdson, D. (1994-01-01). Olney, P. J. S.; Mace, G. M.; Feistner, A. T. C., eds. The role of environmental enrichment in the captive breeding and reintroduction of endangered species. Springer Netherlands. pp. 167–177. ISBN 9789401043113. doi:10.1007/978-94-011-0721-1_8.
  16. 1 2 Moberg, Gary P.; Mench, Joy A. (2000-01-01). The Biology of Animal Stress: Basic Principles and Implications for Animal Welfare. CABI. ISBN 9780851999302.
  17. Mason, G.J., (1991). Stereotypies - A critical review. Animal Behaviour, 41: 1015-1037. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80640-2
  18. Claes, A., Attur Shanmugam, A. and Jensen, P. (2010). Habituation to environmental enrichment in captive Sloth Bears-effect on stereotypies. Zoo Biology, 29: 705-714. doi:10.1002/zoo.20301
  19. Mendl, M., Burman, O.H.P., Parker, R.M.A. and Paul, E.S., (2009). Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 118: 161–181
  20. Duncan, I.J.H. and Wood-Gush, D.G.M., (1971). Frustration and aggression in the domestic fowl. Animal Behaviour, 19:500–504
  21. Zimmerman, P.H., Lundberg, A., Keeling, L.J. and Koene, P., (2003). The effect of an audience on the gakel-call and other frustration behaviours in the laying hen (Gallus gallus domesticus). Animal Welfare, 12: 315–326
  22. Kemppinen, N., Hau, J., Meller, A., Mauranen, K.,Kohila, T. and Nevalainen, T., (2010). Impact of aspen furniture and restricted feeding on activity, blood pressure, heart rate and faecal corticosterone and immunoglobulin A excretion in rats (Rattus norvegicus) housed in individually ventilated cages. Laboratory Animals, 44: 104-112
  23. Laws, N., Ganswindt, A., Heistermann, M., Harris, M., Harris, S. and Sherwin, C., (2007). A case study: fecal corticosteroid and behavior as indicators of welfare during relocation of an asian elephant. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 10: 349-358. doi:10.1080/10888700701555600
  24. Martin L.B., Kidd, L., Liebl A.L. and Coon, C.A.C., (2011). Captivity induces hyper-inflammation in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 214: 2579-2585. doi:10.1242/jeb.057216
  25. Lewis M.H., Presti M.F., Lewis J.B. and Turner, C.A., (2006). The neurobiology of stereotypy I: Environmental complexity. In Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare, G. Mason and J. Rushen (Editors). CABI. pp. 190-226. doi:10.1079/9780851990040.0190
  26. Hughes, B.O., Gilbert, A.B. and Brown, M.F., (1986). Categorisation and causes of abnormal egg shells: relationship with stress. British Poultry Science, 27: 325-337
  27. Wilcox, C.S., Patterson, J. and Cheng, H.W., (2009). Use of thermography to screen for subclinical bumblefoot in poultry. Poultry Science, 88: 1176-1180. doi:10.3382/ps.2008-00446
  28. Richard Crawford (2007). "A Quick Reference to the Requirement for Environmental Enhancement for Primates Under the Animal Welfare Act". Archived from the original on 20 November 2007. Retrieved 2007-11-06.
  29. Richard L. Crawford (2007). "A Quick Reference to the Requirement for the Exercise of Dogs Under the Animal Welfare Act". Archived from the original on 20 November 2007. Retrieved 2007-11-06.
  30. "U.S. Laws, Regulations and Guidelines for Environmental Enhancement of Nonhuman Primates". USDA, Animal Welfare Information Center. 2006. Archived from the original on 2009-04-26. Retrieved 2007-11-06.
  31. Kulpa-Eddy, Jodie A.; Taylor, Sylvia; Adams, Kristina M. (2005), "USDA Perspective on Environmental Enrichment for Animals" (PDF), ILAR Journal, Washington, DC: Institute for Laboratory Animal Research, 26 (2): 83–94, ISSN 0018-9960, archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-29
  32. "Health, Husbandry, and Welfare". www.aza.org. 2013-02-12. Archived from the original on 2016-05-16. Retrieved 2016-04-25.
  33. "How Does Accreditation Work?". www.aza.org. 2015-11-03. Retrieved 2016-04-25.
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Behavioral enrichment (animals).
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.