Social comparison theory

Social comparison theory, initially proposed by social psychologist Leon Festinger in 1954,[1] centers on the belief that there is a drive within individuals to gain accurate self-evaluations. The theory explains how individuals evaluate their own opinions and abilities by comparing themselves to others in order to reduce uncertainty in these domains, and learn how to define the self.

Following the initial theory, research began to focus on social comparison as a way of self-enhancement,[2][3] introducing the concepts of downward and upward comparisons and expanding the motivations of social comparisons.[4]

Initial framework

In the initial theory, Festinger provided nine main hypotheses. First, he stated that humans have a basic drive to evaluate their opinions and abilities and that people evaluate themselves through objective, nonsocial means (Hypothesis I).[1] Second, Festinger stated that if objective, nonsocial means were not available, that people evaluate their opinions and abilities by comparison to other people (Hypothesis II).[1] Next, he hypothesized that the tendency to compare oneself to another person decreases as the difference between their opinions and abilities becomes more divergent.[1] In other words, if someone is much different from you, you are less likely to compare yourself to that person (Hypothesis III). He next hypothesized that there is a unidirectional drive upward in the case of abilities, which is largely absent in opinions.[1] This drive refers to the value that is placed on doing better and better.[5] (Hypothesis IV). Next, Festinger hypothesizes that there are non-social restraints that make it difficult or even impossible to change one’s ability and these restraints are largely absent for opinions.[1] In other words, people can change their opinions when they want to but no matter how motivated individuals may be to improve their ability, there may be other elements that make this impossible[5] (Hypothesis V). Festinger goes on to hypothesize that the cessation of comparison with others is accompanied by hostility or derogation to the extent that continued comparison with those persons implies unpleasant consequences (Hypothesis VI). Next, any factors which increase the importance of some particular group as a comparison group from some particular opinion or ability will increase the pressure toward uniformity concerning that ability or opinion within that group. If discrepancies arise between the evaluator and comparison group there is a tendency to reduce the divergence by either attempting to persuade others, or changing their personal views to attain uniformity. However, the importance, relevance and attraction to a comparison group that affects the original motivation for comparison, mediates the pressures towards uniformity (Hypothesis VII). His next hypothesis states that if persons who are very divergent from one’s own opinion or ability are perceived as different from oneself on attributes consistent with the divergence, the tendency to narrow the range of comparability becomes stronger (Hypothesis VIII). Lastly, Festinger hypothesized that when there is a range of opinion or ability in a group, the relative strength of the three manifestations of pressures toward uniformity will be different for those who are close to the mode of the group than for those who are distant from the mode. Those close to the mode will have stronger tendencies to change the positions of others, weaker tendencies to narrow the range of comparison, and even weaker tendencies to change their own opinions (Hypothesis IX).[1]

Theoretical advances

Since its inception, the initial framework has undergone several advances. Key among these are developments in understanding the motivations that underlie social comparisons, and the particular types of social comparisons that are made. Motives that are relevant to social comparison include self-enhancement,[2][3] maintenance of a positive self-evaluation,[6] components of attributions and validation,[7] and the avoidance of closure.[8][9] While there have been changes in Festinger's original concept, many fundamental aspects remain, including the prevalence of the tendency towards social comparison and the general process that is social comparison.

Self-evaluation

According to Thorton and Arrowood, self-evaluation is one of the functions of social comparison. This is one process that underlies how an individual engages in social comparison.[10] Each individual's specific goals will influence how they engage in social comparison. For self-evaluation, people tend to choose a comparison target that is similar to themselves.[11] Specifically, they are most interested in choosing a target who shares some distinctive characteristic with themselves. Research suggests that most people believe that choosing a similar target helps ensure the accuracy of the self-evaluation. However, individuals do not always act as unbiased self-evaluators, and accurate self-evaluations may not be the primary goal of social comparison.

Self-enhancement

Individuals may also seek self-enhancement, or to improve their self-esteem.[11] They may interpret, distort, or ignore the information gained by social comparison to see themselves more positively and further their self-enhancement goals. They will also choose to make upward (comparing themselves to someone better off) or downward (comparing themselves to someone worse off) comparisons, depending on which strategy will further their self-enhancement goals. They may also avoid making comparisons period, or avoid making certain types of comparisons. Specifically, when an individual believes that their ability in a specific area is low, they will avoid making upward social comparisons in that area. Unlike for self-evaluation goals, people engaging in social comparison with the goal of self-enhancement may not seek out a target that is similar to themselves. In fact, if a target's similarity is seen as a threat, due to the target outperforming the individual on some dimension, the individual may downplay the similarity of the target to themselves.

Later advances in theory led to self-enhancement being one of the four self-evaluation motives:, along with self-assessment, self-verification, and self-improvement.

Main article: Self-enhancement
Main article: Self-assessment
Main article: Self-verification
Main article: Self-improvement

Upward and downward social comparisons

Wills introduced the concept of downward comparison in 1981.[3] Downward social comparison is a defensive tendency that is used as a means of self-evaluation. When a person looks to another individual or group that they consider to be worse off than themselves in order to feel better about their self or personal situation, they are making a downward social comparison. Research has suggested that social comparisons with others who are better off or superior, or upward comparisons, can lower self-regard,[12] whereas downward comparisons can elevate self-regard.[13] Downward comparison theory emphasizes the positive effects of comparisons in increasing one’s subjective well-being.[3] For example, it has been found that breast cancer patients made the majority of comparisons with patients less fortunate than themselves.[14]

Although social comparison research has suggested that upward comparisons can lower self-regard, Collins indicates that this is not always the case.[15] Individuals make upward comparisons, whether consciously or subconsciously, when they compare themselves with an individual or comparison group that they perceive as superior or better than themselves in order to improve their views of self or to create a more positive perception of their personal reality. Upward social comparisons are made to self-evaluate and self-improve in the hopes that self-enhancement will also occur. In an upward social comparison, people want to believe themselves to be part of the elite or superior, and make comparisons highlighting the similarities between themselves and the comparison group, unlike a downward social comparison, where similarities between individuals or groups are disassociated.[8]

It has also been suggested that upward comparisons may provide an inspiration to improve, and in one study it was found that while breast cancer patients made more downward comparisons, they showed a preference for information about more fortunate others.[16]

Another study indicated that people who were dieting often used upward social comparisons by posting pictures of thinner people on their refrigerators.[15] These pictures served as not only a reminder of an individuals current weight, but also as an inspiration of a goal to be reached. In simple terms, downward social comparisons are more likely to make us feel better about ourselves, while upward social comparisons are more likely to motivate us to achieve more or reach higher.

Moderators of social comparison

Aspinwall and Taylor looked at mood, self-esteem, and threat as moderators that drive individuals to choose to make upward or downward social comparisons.[17] Downward comparisons in cases where individuals had experienced a threat to their self-esteem produced more favorable self-evaluations.

High self-esteem and social comparison

Aspinwall and Taylor found that upward social comparisons were good in circumstances where the individuals making the comparisons had high self-esteem, because these types of comparisons provided them with more motivation and hope than downward social comparisons.[17] However, if these individuals had experienced a recent threat or setback to their self-esteem, they reported that upward comparisons resulted in a more negative affect than downward comparisons.

Low self-esteem and social comparison

However, people with low self-esteem or people who are experiencing some sort of threat in their life (such as doing poorly in school, or suffering from an illness) tend to favor downward comparisons over upward comparisons. People with low self-esteem and negative affect improve their mood by making downward comparisons. Their mood does not improve as much as it would if they had high self-esteem. Even for people with low self-esteem, these downward social comparisons do improve their negative mood and allow them to feel hope and motivation for their future.

Affect/mood and its effect on social comparison

Individuals who have a negative mood improve their mood by making upward social comparisons, regardless of their level of self-esteem. In addition, both individuals with high self-esteem and low self-esteem who are in a positive mood elevate their mood further by making upward comparisons. However, for those who have recently experienced a threat to their self-esteem or a setback in their life, making upward social comparisons instead of downward social comparisons results in a more negative affect. Self-esteem and existence of a threat or setback in an individual's life are two moderators of their response to upward or downward comparisons.

Competitiveness

Because individuals are driven upwards in the case of abilities, social comparisons can drive competition among peers.[18] In this regard, the psychological significance of a comparison depends on the social status of an individual, and the context in which their abilities are being evaluated.

Social status

Competitiveness resulting from social comparisons may be greater in relation to higher social status because individuals with more status have more to lose. In one study, students in a classroom were presented with a bonus point program where, based on chance, the grades for some students would increase and the grades for others would remain the same. Despite the fact that students could not lose by this program, higher-status individuals were more likely to object to the program, and more likely to report a perceived distributive injustice. It was suggested that this was a cognitive manifestation of an aversion to downward mobility, which has more psychological significance when an individual has more status.[19]

Proximity to a standard

When individuals are evaluated where meaningful standards exist, such as in an academic classroom where students are ranked, then competitiveness increases as proximity to a standard of performance increases. When the only meaningful standard is the top, then high-ranking individuals are most competitive with their peers, and individuals at low and intermediate ranks are equally competitive. However, when both high and low rankings hold significance, then individuals at high and low ranks are equally competitive, and are both more competitive than individuals at intermediate ranks.[20][21]

Models of social comparison

Several models have been introduced to social comparison, including the Self-Evaluation Maintenance Model (SEM),[12] Proxy Model,[22] the Triadic Model and the Three-Selves Model.[23]

Self-Evaluation Maintenance Model

The SEM model proposes that we make comparisons to maintain or enhance our self-evaluations, focusing on the antagonistic processes of comparison and reflection.[12] Abraham Tesser has conducted research on self-evaluation dynamics that has taken several forms. A self-evaluation maintenance (SEM) model of social behavior focuses on the consequences of another person’s outstanding performance on one’s own self-evaluation. It sketches out some conditions under which the other’s good performance bolsters self-evaluation, i.e., "basking in reflected glory", and conditions under which it threatens self-evaluation through a comparison process.[24]

Proxy Model

The Proxy Model anticipates the success of something that is unfamiliar. The model proposes that if a person is successful or familiar with a task, then he or she would also be successful at a new similar task. The proxy is evaluated based on ability and is concerned with the question "Can I do X?" A proxy's comparison is based previous attributes. The opinion of the comparer and whether the proxy exerted maximum effort on a preliminary task are variables influencing his or her opinion.[8]

Triadic Model

The Triadic Model builds on the attribution elements of social comparison, proposing that opinions of social comparison are best considered in terms of 3 different evaluative questions: preference assessment (i.e., “Do I like X?”), belief assessment (i.e., “Is X correct?”), and preference prediction (i.e., “Will I like X?”). In the Triadic Model the most meaningful comparisons are with a person who has already experienced a proxy and exhibits consistency in related attributes or past preferences.[8]

Three-Selves Model

The Three-Selves Model proposes that social comparison theory is a combination of two different theories. One theory is developed around motivation and the factors that influence the type of social comparison information people seek from their environment and the second is about self-evaluation and the factors that influence the effects of social comparisons on the judgments of self.[23] While there has been much research in the area of comparison motives, there has been little in the area of comparative evaluation. Explaining that the self is conceived as interrelated conceptions accessible depending upon current judgment context[25] and taking a cue from Social Cognitive Theory, this model examines the Assimilation effect and distinguishes three classes of working Self-concept ideas: individual selves, possible selves and collective selves.

Media influence

The media has been found to play a large role in social comparisons. Researchers examining the social effects of the media have used social comparison theory have found that in most cases women tend to engage in upward social comparisons with a target other, which results in more negative feelings about the self. The majority of women have a daily opportunity to make upward comparison by measuring themselves against some form of societal ideal. Social comparisons have become a relevant mechanism for learning about the appearance-related social expectations among peers and for evaluating the self in terms of those standards” (Jones, 2001, P. 647).

Although men do make upward comparisons, research finds that more women make upward comparisons and are comparing themselves with unrealistically high standards presented in the media.[26] As women are shown more mainstream media images of powerful, successful and thin women, they perceive the “ideal” to be the norm for societal views of attractive. Some women have reported making upward comparisons in a positive manner for the purposes of self-motivation, but the majority of upward comparisons are made when the individual is feeling lesser and therefore evoke a negative connotation.

Criticisms

Many criticisms arose regarding Festinger’s similarity hypothesis. Deutsch and Krauss[27] argued that people actually seek out dissimilar others in their comparisons maintaining that this is important for providing valuable self-knowledge, as demonstrated in research.[28][29] Ambiguity also circulated about the important dimensions for similarity. Goethals and Darley clarified the role of similarity suggesting that people prefer to compare those who are similar on related attributes such as opinions, characteristics or abilities to increase confidence for value judgments, however those dissimilar in related attributes are preferred when validating one’s beliefs.[7]

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Festinger L (1954). "A theory of social comparison processes". Human relations 7 (2): 117–140. doi:10.1177/001872675400700202.
  2. 1 2 Gruder C. L. (1971). "Determinants of social comparison choices". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 7 (5): 473–489. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(71)90010-2.
  3. 1 2 3 4 Wills T. A. (1981). "Downward comparison principles in social psychology". Psychological Bulletin 90 (2): 245–271. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.245.
  4. Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation: Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness (Vol. 1). Stanford University Press.
  5. 1 2 Suls, J., Miller, R. (1977). "Social Comparison Processes: Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives". Hemisphere Publishing Corp., Washington D.C. ISBN 0-470-99174-7
  6. Tesser, A.; Campbell, J. (1982). "Self-evaluation maintenance and the perception of friends and strangers.". Journal of Personality 50 (3): 261–279. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1982.tb00750.x.
  7. 1 2 Goethals, G. R.; Darley, J. (1977). "Social comparison theory: An attributional approach.". Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives: 86–109.
  8. 1 2 3 4 Suls, J.; Martin, R.; Wheeler, L. (2002). "Social comparison: Why, with whom, and with what effect?.". Current Directions in Psychological Science 11 (5): 159–163. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00191.
  9. Kruglanski, A. W.; Mayseless, O. (1990). "Classic and current social comparison research: Expanding the perspective.". Psychological Bulletin 108 (2): 195–208. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.195.
  10. Thorton, D.; Arrowood, A. J. (1966). "Self-evaluation, self-enhancement, and the locus of social comparison.". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 2: 591–605. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(69)90049-3.
  11. 1 2 Wood, J. V. (1989). "Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes.". Psychological Bulletin 106 (2): 231–248. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231.
  12. 1 2 3 Tesser, A.; Millar, M.; Moore, J. (1988). "Some affective consequences of social comparison and reflection processes: the pain and pleasure of being close.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 (1): 49–61. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.49.
  13. Gibbons, F. X. (1986). "Social comparison and depression: Company's effect on misery.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51 (1): 140–148. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.1.140.
  14. Wood, J. V.; Taylor, S. E.; Lichtman, R. R. (1985). "Social comparison in adjustment to breast cancer.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49 (5): 1169–1183. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.5.1169.
  15. 1 2 Collins, R. L. (1995). "For better or worse: The impact of upward social comparison on self-evaluations.". Psychological Bulletin 119 (1): 51–69. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.51.
  16. Taylor, S. E.; Lobel, M. (1989). "Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts.". Psychological Review 96 (4): 569–575. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.569.
  17. 1 2 Aspinwall, L. G.; Taylor, S. E. (1993). "Effects of social comparison direction, threat, and self-esteem on affect, self-evaluation, and expected success.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64 (5): 708–722. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.708.
  18. Chen, P. & Garcia, S. M. (manuscript) "Yin and Yang Theory of Competition: Social Comparison and Evaluation Apprehension Reciprocally Drive Competitive Motivation". link.
  19. Burleigh T. J., Meegan D. V. (2013). "Keeping up with the Joneses affects perceptions of distributive justice" (PDF). Social Justice Research 26: 120–131. doi:10.1007/s11211-013-0181-3.
  20. Garcia S. M., Tor A. (2007). "Rankings, standards, and competition: Task vs. scale comparisons". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 102 (1): 95–108. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.10.004.
  21. Garcia S. M., Tor A., Gonzalez R. (2006). "Ranks and rivals: a theory of competition". Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 32 (7): 970–82. doi:10.1177/0146167206287640.
  22. Wheeler L., Martin R., Suls J. (1997). "The proxy model of social comparison for self-assessment of ability". Personality and Social Psychology Review 1 (1): 54–61. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_4.
  23. 1 2 Blanton, H. (2001). Evaluating the self in the context of another: The three-selves model of social comparison assimilation and contrast. In Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 75-87). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  24. Tesser, A., Social Psychology Network; http://tesser.socialpsychology.org/
  25. Markus H., Wurf E. (1987). "The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological perspective". Annual Review of Psychology 38 (1): 299–337. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.38.1.299.
  26. Strahan E. J., Wilson A. E., Cressman K. E., Buote V. M. (2006). "Comparing to perfection: How cultural norms for appearance affect social comparisons and self-image". Body Image 3 (3): 211–227. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.07.004.
  27. Deutsch, M., & Krauss, R. M. (1965). Theories in social psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Basic Books.
  28. Goethals G. R., Nelson R. E. (1973). "Similarity in the influence process: The belief-value distinction". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 25 (1): 117–122. doi:10.1037/h0034266.
  29. Mettee, D. R., & Smith, G. (1977). Social comparison and interpersonal attraction: The case for dissimilarity. Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives, 69, 101.

Further reading

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Monday, February 15, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.