Race and intelligence
Race |
---|
Genetics and differences |
Society |
Race and... |
Related topics |
|
The connection between race and intelligence has been a subject of debate in both popular science and academic research since the inception of IQ testing in the early 20th century. The debate concerns the interpretation of research findings that test takers identifying as "White" tend on average to score higher than test takers of African ancestry on IQ tests, and subsequent findings that test takers of East Asian background tend to score higher than whites. It is still not resolved what relation, if any, there is between group differences in IQ and race.[1]
The first test showing differences in IQ test results between different population groups in the US was the tests of United States Army recruits in World War I. In the 1920s groups of eugenics lobbyists argued that this demonstrated that African-Americans and certain immigrant groups were of inferior intellect to Anglo-Saxon whites due to innate biological differences, using this as an argument for policies of racial segregation. Soon, other studies appeared, contesting these conclusions and arguing instead that the Army tests had not adequately controlled for the environmental factors such as socio-economic and educational inequality between African-Americans and Whites. The debate reemerged again in 1969, when Arthur Jensen championed the view that for genetic reasons Africans were less intelligent than whites and that compensatory education for African-American children was therefore doomed to be ineffective. In 1994, the book The Bell Curve, argued that social inequality in America could largely be explained as a result of IQ differences between races and individuals rather than being their cause, and rekindled the public and scholarly debate with renewed force. During the debates following the book's publication the American Anthropological Association and the American Psychological Association (APA) published official statements regarding the issue, both highly skeptical of some of the book's claims, although the APA report called for more empirical research on the issue.
In subsequent decades much research has been published about the relationships between hereditary influences on IQ, group differences in intelligence, race, environmental influences on IQ. Particularly contentious in the ongoing debate has been the definition of both the concept "race" and the concept "intelligence", and especially whether they can in fact be objectively defined and operationalized. While several environmental factors have been shown to affect group differences in intelligence, it has not been demonstrated that they can explain the entire disparity. On the other hand, no genetic factor has been conclusively shown to have a causal relation with group difference in intelligence test scores. Recent summaries of the debate call for more research into the topic to determine the relative contributions of environmental and genetic factors in explaining the apparent IQ disparity among racial groups.
History of the debate
Claims of races having different intelligence were used to justify colonialism, slavery, racism, social Darwinism, and racial eugenics. Racial thinkers such as Arthur de Gobineau relied crucially on the assumption that black people were innately inferior to Whites in developing their ideologies of White supremacy. Even enlightenment thinkers such as Thomas Jefferson, a slave owner, believed Blacks to be innately inferior to Whites in physique and intellect.[2]
Early IQ testing
The first practical intelligence test was developed between 1905 and 1908 by Alfred Binet in France for school placement of children. Binet warned that results from his test should not be assumed to measure innate intelligence or used to label individuals permanently.[3] Binet's test was translated into English and revised in 1916 by Lewis Terman (who introduced IQ scoring for the test results) and published under the name the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales. As Terman's test was published, there was great concern in the United States about the abilities and skills of recent immigrants. Different immigrant nationalities were sometimes thought to belong to different races, such as Slavs. A different set of tests developed by Robert Yerkes were used to evaluate draftees for World War I, and researchers found that people from southern and eastern Europe scored lower than native-born Americans, that Americans from northern states had higher scores than Americans from southern states, and that Black Americans scored lower than white Americans.[4] The results were widely publicized by a lobby of anti-immigration activists, including the New York patrician and conservationist Madison Grant, who considered the nordic race to be superior, but under threat of immigration by inferior breeds. In his influential work A Study of American Intelligence psychologist Carl Brigham used the results of the Army tests to argue for a stricter immigration policy, limiting immigration to countries considered to belong to the "nordic race".[5]
In the 1920s, states like Virginia enacted eugenic laws, such as its 1924 Racial Integrity Act, which established the one-drop rule as law. On the other hand, many scientists reacted to eugenicist claims linking abilities and moral character to racial or genetic ancestry. They pointed to the contribution of environment to test results (such as speaking English as a second language).[6] By the mid-1930s, many United States psychologists adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role in IQ test results, among them Carl Brigham who repudiated his own previous arguments, on the grounds that he realized that the tests were not a measure of innate intelligence. Discussion of the issue in the United States also influenced German Nazi claims of the "nordics" being a "master race", influenced by Grant's writings.[7] As the American public sentiment shifted against the Germans, claims of racial differences in intelligence increasingly came to be regarded as problematic.[8] Anthropologists such as Franz Boas, and Ruth Benedict and Gene Weltfish, did much to demonstrate the unscientific status of many of the claims about racial hierarchies of intelligence.[9] Nonetheless a powerful eugenics and segregation lobby funded largely by textile-magnate Wickliffe Draper, continued to publicize studies using intelligence studies as an argument for eugenics, segregation, anti-immigration legislation.[10]
The Jensenism debates
As the de-segregation of the American South was begun in the 1950s the debate about Black intelligence resurfaced. Audrey Shuey, funded by Draper's Pioneer fund, published a new analysis of Yerkes' tests, concluding that blacks really were of inferior intellect to whites. This study was used by segregationists as an argument that it was to the advantage of Black children to be educated separately from the superior White children.[11] In the 1960s, the debate was further revived as Nobel laureate William Shockley, publicly defended the argument that Black children were innately unable to learn as well as White children.[12] Arthur Jensen stimulated scholarly discussion of the issue with his Harvard Education Review article, "How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?"[13][10][14][15] Jensen's article questioned remedial education for African-American children; he suggested their poor educational performance reflected an underlying genetic cause rather than lack of stimulation at home.[16] Jensen continued to publish on the issue until his death in 2012.
The Bell Curve debate
Another revival of public debate followed the appearance of The Bell Curve (1994), a book by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, who strongly emphasized the societal effects of low IQ (focusing in most chapters strictly on the non-Hispanic white population of the United States).[17] In 1994 a group of 52 researchers (mostly psychologists) signed an editorial statement "Mainstream Science on Intelligence" in response to the book. The Bell Curve also led to a 1995 report from the American Psychological Association, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns", acknowledging a difference between mean IQ scores of whites and blacks as well as the absence of any adequate explanation of it, either environmental or genetic. The Bell Curve prompted the publication of several multiple-author books responding from a variety of points of view.[18][19] They include The Bell Curve Debate (1995), Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth (1996) and a second edition of The Mismeasure of Man (1996) by Stephen Jay Gould.[19] Jensen's last book-length publication, The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability was published a few years later in 1998.
The review article "Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability" by Rushton and Jensen was published in 2005.[20] The article was followed by a series of responses, some in support, some critical.[8][21] Richard Nisbett, another psychologist who had also commented at the time, later included an amplified version of his critique as part of the book Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count (2009).[22] Rushton and Jensen in 2010 made a point-by-point reply to this thereafter.[23] A comprehensive review article on the issue was published in the journal American Psychologist in 2012.[24]
Some of the authors proposing genetic explanations for group differences have received funding from the Pioneer Fund which was headed by Rushton until his death in 2012.[10][19][25][26][27] The Southern Poverty Law Center lists the Pioneer Fund as a hate group, citing the fund's history, its funding of race and intelligence research, and its connections with racist individuals.[28] On the other hand, Ulrich Neisser writes that "Pioneer has sometimes sponsored useful research—research that otherwise might not have been done at all."[29] Other researchers have criticized the Pioneer Fund for promoting scientific racism, eugenics and white supremacy.[10][30][31][32]
Validity of race and IQ
Intelligence, IQ, g and IQ tests
The concept of intelligence and the degree to which intelligence is measurable is a matter of debate. While there is some consensus about how to define intelligence, it is not universally accepted that it is something that can be unequivocally measured by a single figure.[33] A recurring criticism is that different societies value and promote different kinds of skills and that the concept of intelligence is therefore culturally variable and cannot be measured by the same criteria in different societies.[33] Consequently, some critics argue that proposed relationships to other variables are necessarily tentative.[34]
In relation to the study of racial differences in IQ test scores it becomes a crucial question what exactly it is that IQ tests measure. Arthur Jensen was a proponent of the view that there is a correlation between scores on all the known types of IQ tests and that this correlation points to an underlying factor of general intelligence, or g. In most conceptions of g it is considered to be fairly fixed in a given individual and unresponsive to training or other environmental influences. In this view test score differences, especially in those tasks considered to be particularly "g-loaded" reflect the test takers innate capability. Other psychometricians argue that, while there may or may not be a general intelligence factor, performance on tests rely crucially on knowledge acquired through prior exposure to the types of tasks that such tests contain. This view would mean that tests cannot be expected to reflect only the innate abilities of a given individual, because the expression of potential will always be mediated by experience and cognitive habits. It also means that comparison of test scores from persons with widely different life experiences and cognitive habits is not an expression of their relative innate potentials.[35]
Race
The biological validity of race is disputed.[36] The current mainstream view in the social sciences and biology is that race is a social construction based on folk ideologies that construct groups based on social disparities and superficial physical characteristics.[37] Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd (2005) state, "Race is a socially constructed concept, not a biological one. It derives from people's desire to classify."[34] The concept of human "races" as natural and separate divisions within the human species has also been rejected by the American Anthropological Association. The official position of the AAA, adopted in 1998, finds that advances in scientific knowledge have made it "clear that human populations are not unambiguous, clearly demarcated, biologically distinct groups" and that "any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations [is] both arbitrary and subjective."[38] However within population genetics there is ongoing debate about whether the social category of "race" can and should be used as a proxy for individual genetic ancestry. With current methods of genetic analysis it is possible to determine the composition of genetic ancestry of an individual with significant precision. This is because different genes occur with different frequencies in different geographically defined populations, and by correlating a large amount of genes through cluster analysis it is probable to determine with high likelihood the geographic origins of an individual through DNA. This suggests to some that the classical socially defined genetic categories really have a biological basis, in the sense that racial categorization is a visual estimate of a person's continental ancestry based on their phenotype—which correlates with genotypical ancestry as determined by DNA tests.
Race in studies of human intelligence is almost always determined using self-reports, rather than based on analyses of the genetic characteristics of the tested individuals. According to psychologist David Rowe, self-report is the preferred method for racial classification in studies of racial differences because classification based on genetic markers alone ignore the "cultural, behavioral, sociological, psychological, and epidemiological variables" that distinguish racial groups.[39] Hunt and Carlson write that "Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition. Tang et al. (2005) applied mathematical clustering techniques to sort genomic markers for over 3,600 people in the United States and Taiwan into four groups. There was almost perfect agreement between cluster assignment and individuals' self-reports of racial/ethnic identification as white, black, East Asian, or Latino."[40] Sternberg and Grigorenko disagree with Hunt and Carlson's interpretation of Tang, "Tang et al.'s point was that ancient geographic ancestry rather than current residence is associated with self-identification and not that such self-identification provides evidence for the existence of biological race."[41]
Anthropologist C. Loring Brace[42] and geneticist Joseph Graves contradict the notion that cluster analysis and the correlation between self-reported race and genetic ancestry support biological race.[43] They argue that while it is possible to find biological and genetic variation corresponding roughly to the groupings normally defined as races, this is true for almost all geographically distinct populations. The cluster structure of the genetic data is dependent on the initial hypotheses of the researcher and the populations sampled. When one samples continental groups, the clusters become continental; if one had chosen other sampling patterns, the clusters would be different. Kaplan 2011 therefore concludes that, while differences in particular allele frequencies can be used to identify populations that loosely correspond to the racial categories common in Western social discourse, the differences are of no more biological significance than the differences found between any human populations (e.g., the Spanish and Portuguese).
Earl B. Hunt agrees that racial categories are defined by social conventions, though he points out that they also correlate with clusters of both genetic traits and cultural traits. Hunt explains that, due to this, racial IQ differences are caused by these variables that correlate with race, and race itself is rarely a causal variable. Researchers who study racial disparities in test scores are studying the relationship between the scores and the many race-related factors which could potentially affect performance. These factors include health, wealth, biological differences, and education.[44]
Group differences
The study of human intelligence is one of the most controversial topics in psychology. It remains unclear whether group differences in intelligence test scores are caused by heritable factors or by "other correlated demographic variables such as socioeconomic status, education level, and motivation."[45] Hunt and Carlson outlined four contemporary positions on differences in IQ based on race or ethnicity. The first is that these reflect real differences in average group intelligence, which is caused by a combination of environmental factors and heritable differences in brain function. A second position is that differences in average cognitive ability between races are caused entirely by social and/or environmental factors. A third position holds that differences in average cognitive ability between races do not exist, and that the differences in average test scores are the result of inappropriate use of the tests themselves. Finally, a fourth position is that either or both of the concepts of race and general intelligence are poorly constructed and therefore any comparisons between races are meaningless.[40]
United States test scores
Rushton & Jensen (2005) wrote that, in the United States, self-identified blacks and whites have been the subjects of the greatest number of studies. They stated that the black-white IQ difference is about 15 to 18 points or 1 to 1.1 standard deviations (SDs), which implies that between 11 and 16 percent of the black population have an IQ above 100 (the general population median). According to Arthur Jensen and J. Philippe Rushton the black-white IQ difference is largest on those components of IQ tests that are claimed best to represent the general intelligence factor g.[46] The 1996 APA report "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" and the 1994 editorial statement "Mainstream Science on Intelligence" gave more or less similar estimates.[47][48] Roth et al. (2001), in a review of the results of a total of 6,246,729 participants on other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, found a difference in mean IQ scores between blacks and whites of 1.1 SD. Consistent results were found for college and university application tests such as the Scholastic Aptitude Test (N = 2.4 million) and Graduate Record Examination (N = 2.3 million), as well as for tests of job applicants in corporate sections (N = 0.5 million) and in the military (N = 0.4 million).[49]
North East Asians have tended to score relatively higher on visuospatial subtests with lower scores in verbal subtests while Ashkenazi Jews score higher in verbal and reasoning subtests with lower scores in visuospatial subtests. The few Amerindian populations who have been systematically tested, including Arctic Natives, tend to score worse on average than white populations but better on average than black populations.[49]
The racial groups studied in the United States and Europe are not necessarily representative samples for populations in other parts of the world. Cultural differences may also factor in IQ test performance and outcomes. Therefore, results in the United States and Europe do not necessarily correlate to results in other populations.[50]
Global variation of IQ scores
A number of studies have compared average IQ scores between the world's nations, finding patterns of difference between continental populations similar to those associated with race. Richard Lynn and Tatu Vanhanen have argued that populations in the third world, particularly populations in Africa, tend to have limited intelligence because of their genetic composition and that, consequently, education cannot be effective in creating social and economic development in third world countries.[51] Lynn and Vanhanen's studies have been severely criticized for relying on low quality data and for choosing sources in ways that seem to be biased severely towards underestimating the average IQ potential of developing nations, particularly in Africa.[52][53][54] Nonetheless there is a general consensus that the average IQ in developing countries is lower than in developed countries, but subsequent research has favored environmental explanations for this fact, such as lack of basic infrastructure related to health and education.
In the 2002 book IQ and the Wealth of Nations, and IQ and Global Inequality in 2006, Richard Lynn and Tatu Vanhanen created estimates of average IQs for 113 nations. They estimated IQs of 79 other nations based on neighboring nations or other via other manners. They saw a consistent correlation between national development and national IQ averages. They found the highest national IQs among Western and East Asian developed nations and the lowest national IQs in the world's least developed nations in Sub-Saharan Africa and Latin America. In a metaanalysis of studies of IQ estimates in Africa, Wicherts, Dolan & van der Maas (2009), p. 10 concluded that Lynn and Vanhanen had relied on unsystematic methodology by failing to publish their criteria for including or excluding studies. They found that Lynn and Vanhanen's exclusion of studies had depressed their IQ estimate for sub-Saharan Africa, and that including studies excluded in "IQ and Global Inequality" resulted in average IQ of 82 for sub-Saharan Africa, lower than the average in Western countries, but higher than Lynn and Vanhanen's estimate of 67. Wicherts at al. conclude that this difference is likely due to sub-Saharan Africa having limited access to modern advances in education, nutrition and health care.[55]
A 2007 meta-analysis by Rindermann found many of the same groupings and correlations found by Lynn and Vanhanen, with the lowest scores in sub-Saharan Africa, and a correlation of .60 between cognitive skill and GDP per capita. Hunt (2010, pp. 437–439) considers Rindermann's analysis to be much more reliable than Lynn and Vanhanen's. By measuring the relationship between educational data and social well-being over time, this study also performed a causal analysis, finding that when nations invest in education this leads to increased well-being later on.[56]
Flynn effect and the closing gap
For the past century raw scores on IQ tests have been rising; this score increase is known as the "Flynn effect," named after Jim Flynn. In the United States, the increase was continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to about 1998 when the gains stopped and some tests even showed decreasing test scores. For example, in the United States the average scores of blacks on some IQ tests in 1995 were the same as the scores of whites in 1945.[57] Flynn has argued that given that these changes take place between one generation and the next it is highly unlikely that genetic factors could account for the increasing scores, which must then be caused by environmental factors. The Flynn Effect has often been used as an argument that the racial gap in IQ test scores must be environmental too, but this is not generally agreed – others have asserted that the two may have entirely different causes. A meta-analysis by Te Nijenhuis and van der Flier (2013) concluded that the Flynn effect and group differences in intelligence were likely to have different causes. They stated that the Flynn effect is caused primarily by environmental factors and that it's unlikely these same environmental factors play an important role in explaining group differences in IQ.[58] The importance of the Flynn effect in the debate over the causes for the IQ gap lies in demonstrating that environmental factors may cause changes in test scores on the scale of 1 SD. This had previously been doubted.
A separate phenomenon from the Flynn effect has been the discovery that the IQ gap has been gradually closing over the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. A 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of blacks and whites closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,[59] which would be a reduction of about one-third. In the same period the educational achievement disparity also diminished.[60] However this was challenged by Rushton & Jensen who claim the difference remains stable.[61] In a 2006 study, Murray agreed with Dickens and Flynn that there has been a narrowing of the difference; "Dickens' and Flynn's estimate of 3–6 IQ points from a base of about 16–18 points is a useful, though provisional, starting point". But he argued that this has stalled and that there has been no further narrowing for people born after the late 1970s.[62] Recent reviews by Flynn and Dickens (2006), Hunt (2011), Mackintosh (2011), Nisbett et al. 2012 accept the gradual closing of the gap as a fact. Nisbett et al. (2012b) cite Dickens & Flynn (2006) who consider arguments to the contrary by Rushton, Jensen and Murray to be erroneous.
Some studies reviewed by Hunt (2010), p. 418 found that rise in the average achievement of African Americans was caused by a reduction in the number of African American students in the lowest range of scores without a corresponding increase in the number of students in the highest ranges. A 2012 review of the literature found that the IQ gap had diminished by 0.33 standard deviations since first reported.[24][63]
Environmental influences on group differences in IQ
The following environmental factors are some of those suggested as explaining a portion of the differences in average IQ between races. These factors are not mutually exclusive with one another, and some may in fact contribute directly to others. Furthermore, the relationship between genetics and environmental factors may be complicated. For example, the differences in socioeconomic environment for a child may be due to differences in genetic IQ for the parents, and the differences in average brain size between races could be the result of nutritional factors.[64] All recent reviews agree that some environmental factors that are unequally distributed between racial groups have been shown to affect intelligence in ways that could contribute to the test score gap. However currently the question is whether these factors can account for the entire gap between white and black test scores, or only part of it. One group of scholars, including Richard Nisbett, James R. Flynn, Joshua Aronson, Diane Halpern, William Dickens, Eric Turkheimer (2012) have argued that the environmental factors so far demonstrated are sufficient to account for the entire gap, Nicholas Mackintosh(2011) considers this a reasonable argument, but argues that probably it is impossible to ever know for sure; Another group including Earl B. Hunt (2010), Arthur Jensen,[20] J. Philippe Rushton and Richard Lynn have argued that this is impossible. Jensen and Rushton consider that it may account for as little as 20% of the gap W.hie Hunt considers this a vast overstatement he nonetheless considers it likely that some portion of the gap will eventually be shown to be caused by genetic factors.
Test bias
A number of studies have reached the conclusion that IQ tests may be biased against certain groups.[65][66][67][68] The validity and reliability of IQ scores obtained from outside the United States and Europe have been questioned, in part because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures.[69][70] Several researchers have argued that cultural differences limit the appropriateness of standard IQ tests in non-industrialized communities.[71][72]
However, a 1996 report by the American Psychological Association states that controlled studies show that differences in mean IQ scores were not substantially due to bias in the content or administration of the IQ tests. Furthermore, the tests are equally valid predictors of future achievement for black and white Americans.[47] This view is reinforced by Nicholas Mackintosh in his 1998 book IQ and Human Intelligence,[73] and by a 1999 literature review by Brown, Reynolds & Whitaker (1999). Today test bias in the sense that some test items systematically give White test takers an unfair advantage because of the way the test has been elaborated is no longer considered a likely cause of the test score gap. However reviews by Hunt (2011) and Mackintosh (2011) do admit the possibility that IQ tests measure a cognitive skill that Blacks have less chance to develop, and that there is in this sense a bias in society that causes one group to perform under their true potential on the tests. But both scholars maintain that there is no evidence that current tests are systemically biased against black test takers.
Stereotype threat and minority status
Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies or by which one is defined; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance.[74] Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups who already score lower on average or are expected to score lower. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups.[75] Psychometrician Nicholas Mackintosh considers that there is little doubt that the effects of stereotype threat contribute to the IQ gap between blacks and whites.[76]
A large number of studies have shown that systemically disadvantaged minorities, such as the African American minority of the United States generally perform worse in the educational system and in intelligence tests than the majority groups or less disadvantaged minorities such as immigrant or "voluntary" minorities.[47] The explanation of these findings may be that children of caste-like minorities, due to the systemic limitations of their prospects of social advancement, do not have "effort optimism", i.e. they do not have the confidence that acquiring the skills valued by majority society, such as those skills measured by IQ tests, is worthwhile. They may even deliberately reject certain behaviors that are seen as "acting white."[64][77][78]
Socioeconomic environment
Different aspects of the Socioeconomic environment in which children are raised have been shown to correlate with part of the IQ gap, but they do not account for the entire gap.[79] Generally the difference between mean test scores of blacks and whites is not eliminated when individuals and groups are matched on SES, suggesting that the relationship between IQ and SES is not simply one in which SES determines IQ. Rather it may be the case that differences in intelligence, particularly parental intelligence, may also cause differences in SES, making separating the two factors difficult.[47] Hunt (2010), p. 428 summarises data showing that, jointly, SES and parental IQ account for the full gap (in populations of young children, after controlling parental IQ and parental SES, the gap is not statistically different from zero). He argues the SES-linked components reflect parental occupation status, mother's verbal comprehension score and parent-child interaction quality. Hunt also reviews data showing that the correlation between home environment and IQ becomes weaker with age.
Other research has focussed on different causes of variation within low SES and high SES groups.[80] [81] [82] In the US, among low-SES groups, genetic differences account for a smaller proportion variance in IQ than among higher SES populations.[83] Such effects are predicted by the bioecological hypothesis – that genotypes are transformed into phenotypes through nonadditive synergistic effects of the environment.[84] Nisbett et al. (2012) suggest that high SES individuals are more likely to be able to develop their full biological potential, whereas low SES individuals are likely to be hindered in their development by adverse environmental conditions. The same review also points out that adoption studies generally are biased towards including only high and high middle SES adoptive families, meaning that they will tend to overestimate average genetic effects. They also note that studies of adoption from lower-class homes to middle-class homes have shown that such children experience a 12 - 18 pt gain in IQ relative to children who remain in low SES homes.[24]
Health and nutrition
Environmental factors including lead exposure,[85] breast feeding,[87] and nutrition[88][89] can significantly affect cognitive development and functioning. For example, iodine deficiency causes a fall, on average, of 12 IQ points.[90] Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth. The first two years of life is the critical time for malnutrition, the consequences of which are often irreversible and include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.[91] The African American population of the United States is statistically more likely to be exposed to many detrimental environmental factors such as poorer neighborhoods, schools, nutrition, and prenatal and postnatal health care.[92][93] Mackintosh points out that for American Blacks infant mortality is about twice as high as for whites, and low birthweight is twice as prevalent. At the same time white mothers are twice as likely to breastfeed their infants, and breastfeeding is highly correlated with IQ for low birthweight infants. In this way a wide number of health related factors that influence IQ are unequally distributed between the two groups.[94]
The Copenhagen consensus in 2004 stated that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that one-third of the total global population are affected by iodine deficiency. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under suffer from anaemia because of insufficient iron in their diets.[95]
Other scholars have found that simply the standard of nutrition has a significant effect on population intelligence, and that the Flynn effect may be caused by increasing nutrition standards across the world.[96] James Flynn has himself argued against this view.[97]
Some recent research has argued that the retardation caused in brain development by infectious diseases, many of which are more prevalent in non-White populations, may be an important factor in explaining the differences in IQ between different regions of the world.[98] The findings of this research, showing the correlation between IQ, race and infectious diseases was also shown to apply to the IQ gap in the US, suggesting that this may be an important environmental factor.[99]
Education
Several studies have proposed that a large part of the gap can be attributed to differences in quality of education.[100] Racial discrimination in education has been proposed as one possible cause of differences in educational quality between races.[101] According to a paper by Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway, teachers' referral decisions for students to participate in gifted and talented educational programs were influenced in part by the students' ethnicity.[102]
The Abecedarian Early Intervention Project, an intensive early childhood education project, was also able to bring about an average IQ gain of 4.4 points at age 21 in the black children who participated in it compared to controls.[87] Arthur Jensen agreed that the Abecedarian project demonstrates that education can have a significant effect on IQ, but also said that no educational program thus far has been able to reduce the black-white IQ gap by more than a third, and that differences in education are thus unlikely to be its only cause.[103]
Rushton and Jensen argue that long-term follow-up of the Head Start Program found large immediate gains for blacks and whites but that these were quickly lost for the blacks although some remained for whites. They argue that also other more intensive and prolonged educational interventions have not produced lasting effects on IQ or scholastic performance.[46] Nisbett argues that they ignore studies such as Campbell & Ramey (1994) which found that at the age 12, 87% black of infants exposed to an intervention had IQs in the normal range (above 85) compared to 56% of controls, and none of the intervention-exposed children were mildly retarded compared to 7% of controls. Other early intervention programs have shown IQ effects in the range of 4–5 points, which are sustained until at least age 8–15. Effects on academic achievement can also be substantial. Nisbett also argues that not only early age intervention can be effective, citing other successful intervention studies from infancy to college.[104]
A series of studies by Fagan and Holland, measured the effect of prior exposure to the kind of cognitive tasks posed in IQ tests on test performance. Assuming that the IQ gap was the result of lower exposure to tasks using the cognitive functions usually found in IQ tests among African American test takes, they prepared a group of African Americans in this type of tasks before taking an IQ test. The researchers found that there was no subsequent difference in performance between the African-Americans and White test takers.[105][106] Daley and Onwugbuezie conclude that Fagan and Holland demonstrate that "differences in knowledge between Blacks and Whites for intelligence test items can be erased when equal opportunity is provided for exposure to the information to be tested".[36] A similar argument is made by David Marks who argues that IQ differences correlate well with differences in literacy suggesting that developing literacy skills through education causes an increase in IQ test performance.[107][108]
Research into the possible genetic influences on test score differences
It is well-established that intelligence is highly heritable for individuals, and many different kinds of genetically caused intelligence impairments are known. But the possible relations between genetic differences in intelligence within the normal range are not established. Ongoing research aims to understand the contribution of genes to individual differences in intelligence. Currently there is no non-circumstantial evidence that the test score gap has a genetic component,[109][24] although some researchers believe that the existing circumstantial evidence makes it plausible to believe that hard evidence for a genetic component will eventually appear.[110] Several lines of investigation have been followed in the attempt to ascertain whether there is a genetic component to the test score gap as well as its relative contribution to the magnitude of the gap.
Genetics of race and intelligence
The decoding of the human genome has enabled scientists to search for sections of the genome that may contribute to cognitive abilities.
Geneticist, Alan R. Templeton argues that the question about the possible genetic effects on the test score gap is muddled by the general focus on "race" rather than on populations defined by gene frequency or by geographical proximity, and by the general insistence on phrasing the question in terms of heritability.[111] Templeton points out that racial groups neither represent sub-species nor distinct evolutionary lineages, and that therefore there is no basis for making claims about the general intelligence of races.[111] From this point of view the search for possible genetic influences on the black-white test score gap is a priori flawed, because there is no genetic material shared by all Africans or by all Europeans. Mackintosh (2011) points out that by using genetic cluster analysis to correlate gene frequencies with continental populations it could possibly be the case that African populations had a higher frequency of certain genetic variants that contribute to an average lower intelligence. Such a hypothetical situation could hold without all Africans carrying the same genes or belonging to a single Evolutionary lineage. According to Mackintosh, a biological basis for the gap thus cannot be ruled out on a priori grounds.
Intelligence is a polygenic trait. This means that intelligence is under the influence of several genes, possibly several thousand. The effect of most individual genetic variants on intelligence is thought to be very small, well below 1% of the variance in g. Current studies using quantitative trait loci have yielded little success in the search for genes influencing intelligence. Robert Plomin is confident that QTLs responsible for the variation in IQ scores exist, but due to their small effect sizes, more powerful tools of analysis will be required to detect them.[112] Others assert that no useful answers can be reasonably expected from such research before an understanding of the relation between DNA and human phenotypes emerges.[93] Several candidate genes have been proposed to have a relationship with intelligence.[113][114] However, a review of candidate genes for intelligence published in Deary, Johnson & Houlihan (2009) failed to find evidence of an association between these genes and general intelligence, stating "there is still almost no replicated evidence concerning the individual genes, which have variants that contribute to intelligence differences".[115]
A 2005 literature review article by Sternberg, Grigorenko and Kidd stated that no gene has been shown to be linked to intelligence, "so attempts to provide a compelling genetic link of race to intelligence are not feasible at this time".[116] Hunt (2010), p. 447 and Mackintosh (2011), p. 344 concurred, both scholars noting that while several environmental factors have been shown to influence the IQ gap, the evidence for a genetic influence has been circumstantial, and according to Mackintosh negligible. Mackintosh however suggests that it may never become possible to account satisfyingly for the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors. The 2012 review by the Nisbett et al. (2012) concluded that "Almost no genetic polymorphisms have been discovered that are consistently associated with variation in IQ in the normal range". Hunt and several other researchers however maintain that genetic causes cannot be ruled out and that new evidence may yet show a genetic contribution to the gap. Hunt concurs with Rushton and Jensen who considered the 100% environmental hypothesis to be impossible. Nonetheless, Nisbett and colleagues (2012) consider the entire IQ gap to be explained by the environmental factors that have thus far been demonstrated to influence it, and Mackintosh does not find this view to be unreasonable.[24]
Heritability within and between groups
Intelligence as tested by IQ tests is generally considered to be highly heritable. Psychometricians have found that intelligence is substantially heritable within populations, with 30–50% of variance in IQ scores in early childhood being attributable to genetic factors in analyzed US populations, increasing to 75–80% by late adolescence.[47][115] In biology heritability is defined as the ratio of variation attributable to genetic differences in an observable trait to the trait's total observable variation. The heritability of a trait describes the proportion of variation in the trait that is attributable to genetic factors within a particular population. A heritability of 1 indicates that variation correlates fully with genetic variation and a heritability of 0 indicates that there is no correlation between the trait and genes at all. In psychological testing heritability tends to be understood as the degree of correlation between the results of a test taker and those of their biological parents. However, since high heritability is simply a correlation between traits and genes, it does not describe the causes of heritability which in humans can be either genetic or environmental.
Therefore, a high heritability measure does not imply that a trait is genetic or unchangeable, however, as environmental factors that affect all group members equally will not be measured by heritability and the heritability of a trait may also change over time in response to changes in the distribution of genes and environmental factors.[47] High heritability also doesn't imply that all of the heritability is genetically determined, but can also be due to environmental differences that affect only a certain genetically defined group (indirect heritability).[117] The figure to the left demonstrates how heritability works. In both gardens the difference between tall and short cornstalks is 100% heritable as cornstalks that are genetically disposed for growing tall will become taller than those without this disposition, but the difference in height between the cornstalks to the left and those on the right is 100% environmental as it is due to different nutrients being supplied to the two gardens. Hence the causes of differences within a group and between groups may not be the same, even when looking at traits that are highly heritable.[117]
In regards to the IQ gap the question becomes whether racial groups can be shown to be influenced by different environmental factors that may account for the observed differences between them. Jensen originally argued that given the high heritability of IQ the only way that the IQ gap could be explained as caused by the environment would be if it could be shown that all blacks were subject to a single "x-factor" which affected no white populations while affecting all black populations equally.[118] Jensen considered the existence of such an x-factor to be extremely improbable, but Flynn's discovery of the Flynn effect showed that in spite of high heritability environmental factors could cause considerable disparities in IQ between generations of the same population, showing that the existence of such an x-factor was not only possible but real.[119]
Jensen has also argued that heritability of traits rises with age as the genetic potential of individuals becomes expressed. He sees this as related to the fact that the IQ gap between white and black test takers has been shown to appear gradually, with the gap widening as cohorts reach adulthood. This he sees as a further argument in favor of Spearman's hypothesis (see section below).
In contrast, Dickens and Flynn argued that the conventional interpretation ignores the role of feedback between factors, such as those with a small initial IQ advantage, genetic or environmental, seeking out more stimulating environments which will gradually greatly increase their advantage, which, as one consequence in their alternative model, would mean that the "heritability" figure is only in part due to direct effects of genotype on IQ.[120][121][122]
Today researchers such as Hunt (2010), Nisbett et al. (2012) and Mackintosh (2011) consider that rather than a single factor accounting for the entire gap, probably many different environmental factors differ systematically between the environments of White and Black people converge to create part of the gap and perhaps all of it. They argue that it does not make sense to talk about a single universal heritability figure for IQ, rather, they state, heritability of IQ varies between and within groups. They point specifically to studies showing a higher heritability of test scores in White and medium-high SES families, but considerably lower heritability for Black and low-SES families. This they interpret to mean that children who grow up with limited resources do not get to develop their full genetic potential.[24]
Spearman's hypothesis
Spearman's hypothesis states that the magnitude of the black-white difference in tests of cognitive ability is entirely or mainly a function of the extent to which a test measures general mental ability, or g. The hypothesis was first formalized by Arthur Jensen who devised the statistical Method of Correlated Vectors to test it. Jensen holds that if Spearman's hypothesis holds true then some cognitive tasks have a higher g-load than others, and that these tasks are exactly the tasks in which the gap between Black and White test takers are greatest. Jensen, and other psychometricians such as Rushton and Lynn, take this to show that the cause of g and the cause of the gap are the same—in their view genetic differences.[20]
Mackintosh (2011), pp. 338–39 acknowledges that Jensen and Rushton have shown a modest correlation between g-loading, heritability, and the test score gap, but he does not accept that this demonstrates a genetic origin of the gap. He points out that it is exactly in those the tests that Rushton and Jensen consider to have the highest g-loading and heritability such as the Wechsler that has seen the highest increases due to the Flynn effect. This suggests that they are also the most sensitive to environmental changes. And in turn if the highly g-loaded tests are both more liable to environmental influences and as Jensen argues the ones where the black-white gap is most pronounced, it suggests in fact contrary to Jensen's argument that the gap is most likely caused by environmental factors. Mackintosh also argues that Spearman's hypothesis, which he considers to be likely to be correct, simply shows that the test score gap is based on whatever cognitive faculty is central to intelligence - but not what this factor is. Nisbett et al. (2012), p. 146 make the same point, noting also that the increase in the IQ scores of Black test takers is necessarily also an increase in g.
James Flynn (2012), pp. 140–1 argues that there is an inherent flaw in Jensen's argument that the correlation between g-loadings, test scores and heritability support a genetic cause of the gap. He points out that as the difficulty of a task increases a low performing group will naturally fall further behind, and heritability will therefore also naturally increase. The same holds for increases in performance which will first affect the least difficult tasks, but only gradually affect the most difficult ones. Flynn thus sees the correlation between in g-loading and the test score gap to offer no clue to the cause of the gap.[123]
Hunt (2010), p. 415 states that many of conclusions of Jensen, and his colleagues rest on the validity of Spearman's hypothesis, and the method of correlated vectors used to test it. Hunt points out that other researchers have found this method of calculation to produce false positive results, and that other statistical methods should be used instead. According to Hunt, Jensen and Rushton's frequent claim that Spearman's hypothesis should be regarded as empirical fact does not hold, and that new studies based on better statistical methods would be required to confirm or reject the hypothesis that the correlation between g-loading, heritability and the IQ gap is due to IQ gaps consisting mostly of g.
Adoption studies
A number of studies have been done on the effect of similar rearing conditions on children from different races. The hypothesis is that by investigating whether black children adopted into white families demonstrated gains in IQ test scores relative to black children reared in black families. Depending on whether their test scores are more similar to their biological or adoptive families, that could be interpreted as either supporting a genetic or an environmental hypothesis. The main point of critique in studies like these however whether the environment of black children even when raised in White families are truly comparable to the environment of White children. Several reviews of the adoption study literature has pointed out that it is perhaps impossible to avoid confounding of biological and environmental factors in this type of studies.[124] Given the differing heritability estimates in medium-high SES and low-SES families, Nisbett et al. (2012), pp. 134 argue that adoption studies on the whole tend to overstate the role of genetics because they represent a restricted set of environments, mostly in the medium-high SES range.
The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study (1976) examined the IQ test scores of 122 adopted children and 143 nonadopted children reared by advantaged white families. The children were restudied ten years later.[125][126][127] The study found higher IQ for whites compared to blacks, both at age 7 and age 17.[125] Rushton & Jensen (2005) cite the Minnesota study as providing support to a genetic explanation. Nonetheless, acknowledging the existence of confounding factors, Scarr and Weinberg the authors of the original study, did not themselves consider that it provided support for either the hereditarian or environmentalist view.[128]
Three other adoption studies found contrary evidence to the Minnesota study, lending support to a mostly environmental hypothesis:
- Eyferth (1961) studied the out-of-wedlock children of black and white soldiers stationed in Germany after World War 2 and then raised by white German mothers and found no significant differences.
- Tizard et al. (1972) studied black (African and West Indian), white, and mixed-race children raised in British long-stay residential nurseries. Three out of four tests found no significant differences. One test found higher scores for non-whites.
- Moore (1986) compared black and mixed-race children adopted by either black or white middle-class families in the United States. Moore observed that 23 black and interracial children raised by white parents had a significantly higher mean score than 23 age-matched children raised by black parents (117 vs 104), and argued that differences in early socialization explained these differences.
Rushton and Jensen have argued that unlike the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study, these studies did not retest the children post-adolescence when heritability of IQ would presumably be higher.[23][46] Nisbett (2009, p. 226) however point out that the difference in heritability between ages 7 and 17 are quite small, and that consequently this is no reason to disregard Moore's findings.
Frydman and Lynn (1989) showed a mean IQ of 119 for Korean infants adopted by Belgian families. After correcting for the Flynn effect, the IQ of the adopted Korean children was still 10 points higher than the indigenous Belgian children.[129][20][130]
Reviewing the evidence from adoption studies Mackintosh considers the studies by Tizard and Eyferth to be inconclusive, and the Minnesota study to be consistent only with a partial genetic hypothesis. On the whole he finds that environmental and genetic variables remain confounded and considers evidence from adoption studies inconclusive on the whole, and fully compatible with a 100% environmental explanation.[124]
Racial admixture studies
Most people have an ancestry from different geographic regions, particularly African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors.[131] If racial IQ gaps have a partially genetic basis, one might expect blacks with a higher degree of European ancestry to score higher on IQ tests than blacks with less European ancestry, because the genes inherited from European ancestors would likely include some genes with a positive effect on IQ.[132] Geneticist Alan Templeton has argued that an experiment based on the Mendelian "common garden" design where specimens with different hybrid compositions are subjected to the same environmental influences, would be the only way to definitively show a causal relation between genes and IQ. Summarizing the findings of admixture studies, he concludes that it has shown no significant correlation between any cognitive and the degree of African or European ancestry.[133]
Studies have employed different ways of measuring or approximating relative degrees of ancestry from Africa and Europe. One set of studies have used skin color as a measure, and other studies have used blood groups. Loehlin (2000) surveys the literature and argues that the blood groups studies may be seen as providing some support to the genetic hypothesis, even though the correlation between ancestry and IQ was quite low. He finds that studies by Eyferth (1961), Willerman, Naylor & Myrianthopoulos (1970) did not find a correlation between degree of African&/European ancestry and IQ. The latter study did find a difference based on the race of the mother, with children of white mothers with black fathers scoring higher than children of black mothers and white fathers. Loehlin considers that such a finding is compatible with either a genetic or an environmental cause. All in all Loehlin finds admixture studies inconclusive and recommends more research.
Another study cited by Rushton & Jensen (2005), and by Nisbett et al. (2012), was Moore (1986) study which found that adopted mixed-race children's has test scores identical to children with two black parents - receiving no apparent "benefit" from their white ancestry. Rushton and Jensen find admixture studies to have provided overall support for a genetic explanation though this view is not shared by Loehlin (2000), Nisbett (2009),Hunt (2010), Mackintosh (2011), nor by Nisbett et al. (2012).
Reviewing the evidence from admixture studies Hunt (2010) considers it to be inconclusive because of too many uncontrolled variables. Mackintosh (2011, p. 338) quotes a statement by Nisbett (2009) to the effect that admixture studies have not provided a shred of evidence in favor of a genetic basis for the gap.
Mental chronometry
Mental chronometry measures the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. This reaction time (RT) is considered a measure of the speed and efficiency with which the brain processes information.[134] Scores on most types of RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on standard IQ tests as well as with g, and no relationship has been found between RT and any other psychometric factors independent of g.[134] The strength of the correlation with IQ varies from one RT test to another, but Hans Eysenck gives 0.40 as a typical correlation under favorable conditions.[135] According to Jensen individual differences in RT have a substantial genetic component, and heritability is higher for performance on tests that correlate more strongly with IQ.[136] Nisbett argues that some studies have found correlations closer to 0.2, and that the correlation is not always found.[137]
Several studies have found differences between races in average reaction times. These studies have generally found that reaction times among black, Asian and white children follow the same pattern as IQ scores.[138][139][140] Rushton & Jensen (2005) have argued that reaction time is independent of culture and that the existence of race differences in average reaction time is evidence that the cause of racial IQ gaps is partially genetic instead of entirely cultural. Responding to this argument in Intelligence and How to Get It, Nisbett has pointed to the Jensen & Whang (1993) study in which a group of Chinese Americans had longer reaction times than a group of European Americans, despite having higher IQs. Nisbett also mentions findings in Flynn (1991) and Deary (2001) suggesting that movement time (the measure of how long it takes a person to move a finger after making the decision to do so) correlates with IQ just as strongly as reaction time, and that average movement time is faster for blacks than for whites.[141] Mackintosh (2011), p. 339 considers reaction time evidence unconvincing and points out that other cognitive tests that also correlate well with IQ show no disparity at all, for example the habituation/dishabituation test. And he points out that studies show that rhesus monkeys have shorter reaction times than American college students, suggesting that different reaction times may not tell us anything useful about intelligence.
Brain size
A number of studies have reported a moderate statistical correlation between differences in IQ and brain size between individuals in the same group.[142] [143] And some scholars have reported differences in average brain sizes between Africans, Europeans and Asians. [144] J. P. Rushton has argued that Africans on average have smaller brain cases and brains than Europeans, and that Europeans have smaller brains than East Asians, and that this is evidence that the gap is biological in nature. Critics of Rushton have argued that Rushton's arguments rest on outdated data collected by unsound methods and should be considered invalid.[145] Recent reviews by Nisbett et al. (2012b) and Mackintosh (2011) consider that current data does show an average difference in brain size and head-circumference between American Blacks and Whites, but question whether this has any relevance for the IQ gap. Nesbitt et al. argue that crude brain size is unlikely to be a good measure of IQ; for example, brain size also differs between men and women, but without well documented differences in IQ. At the same time newborn Black children have the same average brain size as Whites, suggesting that the difference in average size could be accounted for by differences in postnatal environment. Several factors that reduce brain size have been demonstrated to disproportionately affect Black children.[63]
Earl Hunt states that brain size is found to have a correlation of about .35 with intelligence among whites and cites studies showing that genes may account for as much as 90% of individual variation in brain size. According to Hunt, race differences in average brain size could potentially be an important argument for a possible genetic contribution to racial IQ gaps. Nonetheless, Hunt notes that Rushton's head size data would account for a difference of .09 standard deviations between Black and White average test scores, less than a tenth of the 1.0 standard deviation gap in average scores that is observed.[120][146]
Policy relevance and ethics
The 1996 report of the APA commented on the ethics of research on race and intelligence.[40] Gray & Thompson (2004) as well as Hunt & Carlson (2007) have also discussed different possible ethical guidelines.[40][147] Nature in 2009 featured two editorials on the ethics of research in race and intelligence by Steven Rose (against) and Stephen J. Ceci and Wendy M. Williams (for).[148][149]
According to critics, research on group differences in IQ will reproduce the negative effects of social ideologies (such as Nazism or social Darwinism) that were justified in part on claimed hereditary racial differences.[38][150] Steven Rose maintains that the history of eugenics makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science.[149]
Linda Gottfredson argues that suggestion of higher ethical standards for research into group differences in intelligence is a double standard applied in order to undermine disliked results.[151] James R. Flynn has argued that had there been a ban on research on possibly poorly conceived ideas, much valuable research on intelligence testing (including his own discovery of the Flynn effect) would not have occurred.[152]
Jensen and Rushton argued that the existence of biological group differences does not rule out, but raises questions about the worthiness of policies such as affirmative action or placing a premium on diversity. They also argued for the importance of teaching people not to overgeneralize or stereotype individuals based on average group differences, because of the significant overlap of people with varying intelligence between different races.[46]
The environmentalist viewpoint argues for increased interventions in order to close the gaps. Nisbett argues that schools can be greatly improved and that many interventions at every age level are possible.[153] Flynn, arguing for the importance of the black subculture, writes that "America will have to address all the aspects of black experience that are disadvantageous, beginning with the regeneration of inner city neighbourhoods and their schools. A resident police office and teacher in every apartment block would be a good start."[154] Researchers from both sides agree that interventions should be better researched.[137][23]
Especially in developing nations, society has been urged to take on the prevention of cognitive impairment in children as of the highest priority. Possible preventable causes include malnutrition, infectious diseases such as meningitis, parasites, cerebral malaria, in utero drug and alcohol exposure, newborn asphyxia, low birth weight, head injuries, lead poisoning and endocrine disorders.[155]
See also
References
- ↑ Sternberg ,Robert J., Elena L. Grigorenko, and Kenneth K. Kidd (2005). "Intelligence, Race, and Genetics". American Psychologist 60 (1): 46 –59. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46. PMID 15641921.
- ↑ Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 23.
- ↑ Plotnik & Kouyoumdjian 2011.
- ↑ Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 116.
- ↑ Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 116, 309.
- ↑ Pickren & Rutherford 2010, p. 163.
- ↑ Spiro 2009.
- 1 2 Ludy 2006
- ↑ Jackson & Weidman 2004, pp. 130–32.
- 1 2 3 4 Tucker 2002.
- ↑ Jackson 2005.
- ↑ Shurkin 2006.
- ↑ Jensen 1969, p. 82.
- ↑ Wooldridge 1995.
- ↑ Panofsky, Aaron (2014). Misbehaving Science. Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-05831-3.
- ↑ Alland 2002, pp. 79–80.
- ↑ Herrnstein & Murray 1994.
- ↑ Mackintosh 1998
- 1 2 3 Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007
- 1 2 3 4 Rushton & Jensen 2005.
- ↑ Rouvroy 2008, p. 86
- ↑ Nisbett 2009, pp. 209–36
- 1 2 3 Rushton & Jensen 2010
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 Nisbett et al. 2012.
- ↑ Graves 2002a.
- ↑ Graves 2002b.
- ↑ Grossman & Kaufman 2001
- ↑ Berlet 2003.
- ↑ Neisser 2004.
- ↑ Pioneer Fund Board
- ↑ Falk 2008, p. 18
- ↑ Wroe 2008, p. 81
- 1 2 Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp. 350–1
- 1 2 Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005
- ↑ Mackintosh 2011, p. 359.
- 1 2 Daley & Onwuegbuzie 2011.
- ↑ Schaefer 2008
- 1 2 AAA 1998
- ↑ Rowe 2005
- 1 2 3 4 Hunt & Carlson 2007
- ↑ Sternberg & Grigorenko 2007
- ↑ Brace 2005
- ↑ Graves 2001
- ↑ Hunt 2010, pp. 408–10
- ↑ Hampshire et al. 2012.
- 1 2 3 4 Rushton & Jensen 2005
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 Neisser et al. 1996 "The differential between the mean intelligence test scores of Blacks and Whites (about one standard deviation, although it may be diminishing) does not result from any obvious biases in test construction and administration, nor does it simply reflect differences in socio-economic status. Explanations based on factors of caste and culture may be appropriate, but so far have little direct empirical support. There is certainly no such support for a genetic interpretation. At present, no one knows what causes this differential."
- ↑ Gottfredson 1997
- 1 2 Roth et al. 2001
- ↑ Niu & Brass 2011
- ↑ Hunt 2010, p. 437-439.
- ↑ Hunt 2010, pp. 426–445.
- ↑ Wicherts, Dolan & van der Maas 2009, p. 10.
- ↑ Barnett, Susan M. and Williams, Wendy (August 2004). "National Intelligence and the Emperor's New Clothes". Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books 49 (4): 389–396. doi:10.1037/004367.
we see an edifice built on layer upon layer of arbitrary assumptions and selective data manipulation. The data on which the entire book is based are of questionable validity and are used in ways that cannot be justified.
- ↑ Wicherts, Dolan & van der Maas 2009.
- ↑ Hunt 2010, p. 440-443.
- ↑ Mackintosh 1998, p. 162
- ↑ te Nijenhuis, Jan; van der Flier, Henk (2013). "Is the Flynn effect on g?: A meta-analysis". Intelligence 41 (6): 802–807. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.001.
- ↑ Dickens & Flynn 2006.
- ↑ Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association
- ↑ Rushton & Jensen 2006.
- ↑ Murray 2006.
- 1 2 Nisbett et al. 2012b.
- 1 2 Neisser et al. 1996.
- ↑ Cronshaw et al. 2006, p. 278
- ↑ Verney et al. 2005
- ↑ Borsboom 2006
- ↑ Shuttleworth-Edwards et al. 2004
- ↑ Richardson 2004
- ↑ Hunt & Wittmann 2008
- ↑ Irvine 1983
- ↑ Irvine & Berry 1988 a collection of articles by several authors discussing the limits of assessment by intelligence tests in different communities in the world. In particular, Reuning (1988) describes the difficulties in devising and administering tests for Kalahari bushmen.
- ↑ Mackintosh 1998, p. 174: "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attainment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority."
- ↑ Aronson, Wilson & Akert 2005
- ↑ Steele, Claude M. (1997). "A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance". American Psychologist 52 (6): 613–629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 9174398.
- ↑ Mackintosh 2011, p. 348.
- ↑ Ogbu 1978.
- ↑ Ogbu 1994.
- ↑ Hunt 2010, p. 428.
- ↑ Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). "Race, social class, and IQ.". Science 174: 1285–95. doi:10.1126/science.174.4016.1285.
- ↑ Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1974). "Some myths about heritability and IQ.". Nature 251: 463–464. doi:10.1038/251463b0.
- ↑ D. C. Rowe. (1994). The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience and Behaviour. Guilford Press. London
- ↑ Kirkpatrick, R. M.; McGue, M.; Iacono, W. G. (2015). "Replication of a gene-environment interaction Via Multimodel inference: additive-genetic variance in adolescents' general cognitive ability increases with family-of-origin socioeconomic status". Behav Genet 45: 200–14. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9698-y.
- ↑ Bronfenbrenner, Urie; Ceci, Stephen J. "Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model.". Psychological Review 101 (4): 568–586. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568.
- 1 2 Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992
- ↑ MMWR 2005
- 1 2 Campbell et al. 2002
- ↑ Ivanovic et al. 2004
- ↑ Saloojee & Pettifor 2001
- ↑ Qian et al. 2005
- ↑ The Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition, 2008.
- ↑ Nisbett 2009, p. 101
- 1 2 Cooper 2005
- ↑ Mackintosh 2011, pp. 343–44.
- ↑ Behrman, Alderman & Hoddinott 2004
- ↑ Colom, R.; Lluis-Font, J. M.; Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2005). "The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis". Intelligence 33: 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010.
- ↑ Flynn, J. R. (2009). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938 to 2008". Economics and Human Biology 7: 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009.
- ↑ Eppig, Fincher & Thornhill 2010
- ↑ Eppig 2011
- ↑ Manly et al. 2002 and Manly et al. 2004
- ↑ Mickelson 2003
- ↑ Elhoweris et al. 2005
- ↑ Miele 2002, p. 133
- ↑ Nisbett 2005, pp. 303–4
- ↑ Fagan, Joseph F; Holland, Cynthia R (2002). "Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ". Intelligence 30 (4): 361–387. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6.
- ↑ Fagan, J.F.; Holland, C.R. (2007). "Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing". Intelligence 35: 319–334. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.009.
- ↑ Marks, D.F. (2010). "IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: An artifact of differences in literacy skills". Psychological Reports 106 (3): 643–664. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664. PMID 20712152.
- ↑ Barry, Scott (2010-08-23). "The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links?". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2014-08-22.
- ↑ Mackintosh 2011, p. 358.
- ↑ Hunt 2010, pp. 436, 447.
- 1 2 Templeton 2001
- ↑ Plomin, Kennedy & Craig 2005, p. 513
- ↑ Zinkstok et al. 2007
- ↑ Dick et al. 2007
- 1 2 Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009
- ↑ Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005, p. 46.
- 1 2 Block 2002
- ↑ Jensen, A.R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger. pp. 456–8. ISBN 0-275-96103-6.
- ↑ Flynn 2012, pp. 134-141.
- 1 2 Hunt & Carlson 2007.
- ↑ Nisbett 2009, p. 212.
- ↑ Dickens & Flynn 2001.
- ↑ Flynn 2010.
- 1 2 Mackintosh 2011, p. 337.
- 1 2 Weinberg, Scarr & Waldman 1992
- ↑ Scarr & Weinberg 1976.
- ↑ Loehlin 2000, p. 185.
- ↑ Scarr & Weinberg 1990.
- ↑ Loehlin 2000, p. 187.
- ↑ Frydman and Lynn (1989). "The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium". Personality and Individual Differences 10 (12): 1323–1325. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8.
- ↑ Bryc et al. 2009
- ↑ Loehlin 2000.
- ↑ Templeton 2001.
- 1 2 Jensen 2006
- ↑ Eysenck 1987
- ↑ Jensen 1998
- 1 2 Nisbett 2009
- ↑ Lynn & Vanhanen 2002.
- ↑ Jensen & Whang 1993.
- ↑ Pesta & Poznanski 2008.
- ↑ Nisbett 2009, pp. 221–2.
- ↑ Deary, Penke & Johnson 2010.
- ↑ McDaniel 2005.
- ↑ Ho et al. 1980.
- ↑ Lieberman 2001.
- ↑ Hunt 2010, pp. 433–434.
- ↑ Gray & Thompson 2004
- ↑ Ceci & Williams 2009
- 1 2 Rose 2009, pp. 786–88
- ↑ American Anthropological Association (1994). "Statement on "Race" and Intelligence". Retrieved March 31, 2010.
- ↑ Gottfredson 2007
- ↑ Flynn 2009
- ↑ Nisbett 2009
- ↑ Flynn 2008
- ↑ Olness 2003
Bibliography
- Alland, Alexander, Jr (2002). Race in Mind. Palgrave Macmillan. pp. 79–80.
- "American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' and Intelligence". American Anthropological Association. December 1994.
- "American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race'". American Anthropological Association. 17 May 1998.
- "AAPA Statement on Biological Aspects of Race". American Journal of Physical Anthropology 101: 569–570. 1996. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1331010408.
- Aronson, E; Wilson, TD; Akert, AM (2005). Social Psychology (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-13-178686-5.
- Behrman, JR; Alderman, H; Hoddinott, J (2004). "Hunger and Malnutrition" (PDF). Copenhagen Consensus.
- Bellinger, David C; Stiles, Karen M; Needleman, Herbert L (December 1992). "Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study". Pediatrics 90 (6): 855–61. PMID 1437425.
- Berlet, Chip (Summer 2003). "Into the Mainstream". Intelligence Report (Southern Poverty Law Center) (110). Retrieved 21 February 2012.
- Block, Ned (2002). "How heritability misleads about race". In Fish, Jefferson. Race and Intelligence: Separating Science from Myth. Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
- Borsboom, Denny (September 2006). "The attack of the psychometricians". Psychometricka 71 (3): 425–40. doi:10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6. PMC 2779444. PMID 19946599.
- Brace, C Loring (1999). "An Anthropological Perspective on 'Race' and Intelligence: The non-clinal nature of human cognitive capabilities". Journal of Anthropological Research 55 (2): 245–64. JSTOR 3631210.
- Brace, C. Loring (2005). Race is a four letter word. Oxford University Press. p. 326. ISBN 978-0-19-517351-2.
- "race". Encyclopædia Britannica (Online ed.). Encyclopædia Britannica Inc. 2012.
- Brown, Robert T; Reynolds, Cecil R; Whitaker, Jean S (1999). "Bias in Mental Testing since "Bias in Mental Testing"". School Psychology Quarterly 14 (3): 208–38. doi:10.1037/h0089007.
- Bryc, Katarzyna; Auton, Adam; Nelson, Matthew R.; Oksenberg, Jorge R.; Hauser, Stephen L.; Williams, Scott; Froment, Alain; Bodo, Jean-Marie; Wambebe, Charles; Tishkoff, Sarah A.; Bustamante, Carlos D. (2009). "Genome-wide patterns of population structure and admixture in West Africans and African Americans". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 (2): 786–91. doi:10.1073/pnas.0909559107. PMC 2818934. PMID 20080753.
- Campbell, FA; Ramey, CT (1994). "Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families". Child Development 65: 684–698. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00777.x.
- Campbell, Frances A; Ramey, Craig T; Pungello, Elizabeth; Sparling, Joseph; Miller-Johnson, Shari (2002). "Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes From the Abecedarian Project". Applied Developmental Science 6: 42–57. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0601_05.
- Ceci, SJ; Williams, WM (2009). "Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? Yes: the scientific truth must be pursued". Nature 457 (7231): 788–9. Bibcode:2009Natur.457..788C. doi:10.1038/457788a. PMID 19212385.
- Cooper, R. S. (2005). "Race and IQ: Molecular Genetics as Deus ex Machina". American Psychologist 60 (1): 71–76. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.71. PMID 15641923.
- Cronshaw, Steven F; Hamilton, Leah K; Onyura, Betty R; Winston, Andrew S (September 2006). "Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons". International Journal of Selection and Assessment 14 (3): 278–287. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x. Check date values in:
|year= / |date= mismatch
(help) - Daley, C. E.; Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2011). "Race and Intelligence". In Sternberg, R.; Kaufman, S. B. The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 293–306. ISBN 9780521518062.
- Deary, Ian J (2001). Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-289321-1.
- Deary, I. J.; Penke, L.; Johnson, W. (2010). "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11 (3): 201–211. doi:10.1038/nrn2793. PMID 20145623.
- Deary, IJ; Johnson, W; Houlihan, LM (2009). "Genetic foundations of human intelligence". Human Genetics 126 (1): 215–32. doi:10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4. PMID 19294424.
- Danielle M., Dick; Fazil, Aliev; John, Kramer; Jen C., Wang; Anthony, Hinrichs; Sarah, Bertelsen; Sam, Kuperman; Marc, Schuckit; John, Jr, Nurnberger; Howard J., Edenberg; Bernice, Porjesz; Henri, Begleiter; Victor, Hesselbrock; Alison, Goate; Laura, Bierut (2007). "Association of CHRM2 with IQ: converging evidence for a gene influencing intelligence". Behavioral Genetics 37 (2): 265–72. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9131-2. PMID 17160701.
- Dickens, William T; Flynn, James R (2001). "Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved". Psychological Review 108 (2): 346–69. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346. PMID 11381833.
- Dickens, William T; Flynn, James R (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples" (PDF). Psychological Science 17 (10): 913–20. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x.
- Elhoweris, Hala; Mutua, Kagendo; Alsheikh, Negmeldin; Holloway, Pauline (2005). "Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs". Remedial and Special Education (PRO-ED, Inc) 26.
- Eppig, Christopher; Fincher, Corey L; Thornhill, Randy (2010). "Parasite prevalence and the worldwide distribution of cognitive ability". Proceedings of the Royal Society B 277 (1701): 3801–8. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0973. PMC 2992705. PMID 20591860.
- Eppig, Christopher (2011). "Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places?". Scientific American.
- Eyferth, K (1961). "Leistungern verscheidener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK)". Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie (in German) 113: 222–41.
- Eysenck, Hans J (1987). "Intelligence and Reaction Time: The Contribution of Arthur Jensen". In Modgil, S; Modgil, C. Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy. New York, NY: Falmer.
- Falk, Avner (2008). Anti-semitism: a history and psychoanalysis of contemporary hatred. Praeger. p. 312. ISBN 978-0-313-35385-7.
- Flynn, James R (1991). "Reaction times show that both Chinese and British children are more intelligent than one another". Perceptual and Motor Skills 72: 544–6. doi:10.2466/pms.1991.72.2.544.
- Flynn, James R (3 September 2008). "Perspectives: Still a question of black vs white?". New Scientist (magazine issue ed.) (2672).
- Flynn, James R (2009). "Would you wish the research undone?" (PDF). Nature 458 (7235): 146. Bibcode:2009Natur.458..146F. doi:10.1038/458146a.
- Flynn, James R (2010). "The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate" (PDF). Intelligence 38 (4): 363–6. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001.
- Flynn, James R. (2012). Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1-107-60917-4. Lay summary (16 May 2013).
- Gottfredson, Linda S (1997). "Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial)" (PDF). Intelligence 24: 13–23. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8.
- Gottfredson, Linda S (2007). "Applying Double Standards to 'Divisive' Ideas: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson". Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 (2): 216–220. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00039.x.
- Gould, Stephen Jay (1981). The Mismeasure of Man. New York, London: Norton. ISBN 0-393-30056-0.
- Graves, Joseph L. (2001). The Emperor's New Clothes: Biological Theories of Race at the Millennium (Kindle ed.). Rutgers University Press. ASIN B000SARS70.
- Graves, Joseph L (2002a). "The Misuse of Life History Theory: JP Rushton and the Pseudoscience of Racial Hierarchy". In Fish, Jefferson M. Race and Intelligence: Separating Myth from Reality. Laurence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 57–94. ISBN 0-8058-3757-4.
- Graves, Joseph L, Jr (2002b). "What a tangled web he weaves: Race, reproductive strategies and Rushton's life history theory". Anthropological Theory 2 (2): 131–54. doi:10.1177/1469962002002002627.
- Gray, Jeremy R; Thompson, Paul M (2004). "Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics" (PDF). Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5 (6): 471–82. doi:10.1038/nrn1405. PMID 15152197.
- Grossman, James B; Kaufman (2001). "Evolutionary Psychology: Promise and Perils". In Sternberg, Robert J; Kaufman, James C. The evolution of intelligence. Routledge. ISBN 0-8058-3267-X.
- Hampshire, A.; Highfield, R. R.; Parkin, B. L.; Owen, A. M. (2012). "Fractionating human intelligence". Neuron 76 (6): 1225–1237. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022.
- Herrnstein, Richard J; Murray, Charles (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press. ISBN 0-02-914673-9.
- Ho, KC; Roessmann, U; Straumfjord, JV; Monroe, G (December 1980). "Analysis of brain weight. I. Adult brain weight in relation to sex, race, and age.". Archives of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine 104 (12): 635–9. PMID 6893659.
- Hunt, Earl (2010). Human Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-70781-7.
- Hunt, Earl; Carlson, Jerry (2007). "Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence". Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 (2): 194–213. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00037.x.
- Hunt, Earl; Wittmann, Werner (January–February 2008). "National intelligence and national prosperity". Intelligence 36 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002.
- Irvine, SH (1983). "Where intelligence tests fail". Nature 302 (5907): 371. Bibcode:1983Natur.302..371I. doi:10.1038/302371b0.
- Irvine, SH; Berry, JW, eds. (1988). "Human Abilities in Cultural Context". Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-34482-4.
- Ivanovic, Daniza M.; Leiva, Boris P.; Pérez, Hernán T.; Olivares, Manuel G.; Dı́az, Nora S.; Urruti, Marı́a Soledad C.; Almagià, Atilio F.; Toro, Triana D.; Miller, Patricio T.; Bosch, Enrique O.; Larraı́n, Cristián G. (2004). "Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain". Neuropsychologia 42 (8): 1118–31. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022. PMID 15093150.
- Jackson, John P.; Weidman, Nadine M. (2004). Race, Racism, and Science: Social Impact and Interaction. ABC-CLIO. p. 23.
- Jackson, John P. (2005). Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case against Brown v. Board of Education. NYU Press. ISBN 978-0-8147-4271-6. Lay summary (30 August 2010).
- Jensen, Arthur R (1969). "How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?". Harvard Educational Review 39: 1–123.
- Jensen, Arthur R (1973). Educability and Group Differences. London: Methuen. ISBN 0-06-012194-7.
- Jensen, Arthur R; Whang, PA (1993). "Reaction times and intelligence: a comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American children". Journal of Biosocial Science 25 (3): 397–410. doi:10.1017/s0021932000020721. PMID 8360233.
- Jensen, Arthur R (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-96103-6.
- Jensen, AR (2006). Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences. Amsterdam: Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-08-044939-5.
- Jensen, Arthur R; Johnson, Fred W (May–June 1994). "Race and sex differences in head size and IQ". Intelligence 18 (3): 309–33. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(94)90032-9.
- Kaplan, Jonathan Michael (January 2011). "'Race': What Biology Can Tell Us about a Social Construct". Encyclopedia of Life Sciences (ELS) (Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd). doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857.
- Kaszycka, Katarzyna A.; Štrkalj, Goran; Strzałko, Jan (2009). "Current Views of European Anthropologists on Race: Influence of Educational and Ideological Background". American Anthropologist (American Anthropological Association) 111 (1): 43–56. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01076.x.
- Lieberman, Leonard (2001). "How "Caucasoids" got such big crania and why they shrank: from Morton to Rushton."". Current Anthropology 42 (1): 69–95. doi:10.1086/318434. PMID 14992214.
- Loehlin, John C (2000). "Group Differences in Intelligence". In Sternberg, Robert J. The Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Ludy, Benjamin T (2006). "Brief History of Modern Psychology". Wiley-Blackwell: 188–91. ISBN 1-4051-3206-X.
- Lynn, Richard; Vanhanen, Tatu (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X.
- Mackintosh, NJ (1998). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-852367-X. Lay summary (9 August 2010).
- Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence (second ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-958559-5. Lay summary (9 February 2012).
- Manly, JJ; Byrd, DA; Touradji, P; Stern, Yaakov (2004). "Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders". Applied Neuropsychology 11 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1207/s15324826an1101_5. PMID 15471745.
- Manly, Jennifer J; Jacobs, Diane M; Touradji, Pegah; Small, Scott A; Stern, Yaakov (2002). "Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 8: 341–348. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813157.
- Maltby, John; Day, Liz; Macaskill, Ann (2007). Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence. Pearson Education. pp. 334–47. ISBN 0-13-129760-0.
- McDaniel, M. A. (2005). "Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence". Intelligence 33 (4): 337–346. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005.
- Mickelson, Roslyn Arlin (2003). "When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective".
- Miele, Frank (2002). Intelligence, Race and Genetics: Conversations with Arthur R. Jensen. Basic Books. p. 256.
- MMWR (27 May 2005). "Blood Lead Levels—United States, 1999–2002". Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR) (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention) 54 (20): 513–6.
- Moore, Elsie G (May 1986). "Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted Black children". Developmental Psychology 22 (3): 317–26. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.317.
- Murray, Charles (2006). "Changes over time in the black–white difference on mental tests: Evidence from the children of the 1979 cohort of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth". Intelligence 34 (6): 527–540. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004.
- Murray, Charles (July–August 2007). "The magnitude and components of change in the black–white IQ difference from 1920 to 1991: A birth cohort analysis of the Woodcock–Johnson standardizations". Intelligence 35 (4): 305–18. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.02.001.
- Neisser, Ulric (2004). "Serious Scientists or Disgusting Racists?". Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books 49 (1): 5–7. doi:10.1037/004224.
- Neisser, Ulric; Boodoo, Gwyneth; Bouchard, Thomas J, Jr; Boykin, A. Wade; Brody, Nathan; Ceci, Stephen J; Halpern, Diane F; Loehlin, John C; Perloff, Robert; Sternberg, Robert J; Urbina, Susana (1996). "Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns" (PDF). American Psychologist 51: 77–101. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77.
- Nisbett, Richard (2005). "Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 11 (2): 302–10. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302.
- Nisbett, Richard (2009). Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count. W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 0-393-06505-7.
- Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments" (PDF). American Psychologist 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22233090. Retrieved 22 July 2013. Lay summary (22 July 2013).
- Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012b). "Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin" (PDF). American Psychologist 67 (6): 503–504. doi:10.1037/a0029772. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22963427. Retrieved 22 July 2013. Lay summary (22 July 2013).
- Niu, Weihua; Brass, Jillian (2011). "Intelligence in Worldwide Perspective". In Sternberg, Robert J; Kaufmann, Scott Barry. The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-51806-7.
- Ogbu, John (1978). "Minority education and caste: The American system in cross-cultural perspective". New York: Academic Press.
- Ogbu, John U (1994). "From cultural differences to differences in cultural frames of reference". In Greenfield, PM; Cocking, RR. Cross-cultural roots of minority child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 365–91.
- Olness, K (April 2003). "Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic". Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 24 (2): 120–30. doi:10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009. PMID 12692458.
- Pesta, Bryan J; Poznanski, Peter J (2008). "Black-White differences on IQ and grades: The mediating role of elementary cognitive tasks". Intelligence 36 (4): 323–29. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.07.004.
- Pickren, Wade E; Rutherford, Alexandra (2010). A History of Modern Psychology in Context. Wiley.
- Plomin, R; Kennedy, J; Craig, I (2005). "The quest for quantitative trait loci associated with intelligence". Intelligence 34 (6): 513–526. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.01.001.
- Plotnik, Rod; Kouyoumdjian, Haig (2011). "Intelligence". Introduction to Psychology. Cengage Learning.
- Posthuma,, Daniëlle; De Geus, Eco JC; Baaré,, Wim FC; Hulshoff Pol, Hilleke E; Kahn, René S; Boomsma, Dorret I (February 2002). "The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin". Nature Neuroscience 5 (2): 83–4. doi:10.1038/nn0202-83. PMID 11818967.
- Qian, M; Wang, D; Watkins, WE; Gebski, V; Yan, YQ; Li, M; Chen, ZP (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706.
- Reuning, Helmut (1988). "Testing Bushmen in the Central Kalahari". In Irvine, SH; Berry, JW. Human Abilities in Cultural Context. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511574603.018. ISBN 978-0-511-57460-3.
- Richardson, K (2004). "Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations". Heredity 92 (4): 359–60. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800418.
- Rose, Steven (2009). "Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit". Nature 457 (7231): 786–8. Bibcode:2009Natur.457..786R. doi:10.1038/457786a. PMID 19212384.
- Roth, PL; Bevier, CA; Bobko, P; Switzer, FS, III; Tyler, P (2001). "Ethnic group differences in cognitive ability in employment and educational settings: A metaanalysis". Personnel Psychology 54: 297–330. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x.
- Rouvroy, Antoinette (2008). "Human genes and neoliberal governance: a Foucauldian critique". Routledge: 86. ISBN 0-415-44433-0.
- Rowe; David C (January 2005). Rodgers, Joseph, ed. "Under the Skin: On the Impartial Treatment of Genetic and Environmental Hypothesis of Racial Differences". American Psychologist 60 (1): 60–70. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60. PMID 15641922.
- Rushton, J. Philippe (1995). Race, evolution, and Behavior. Piscataway, NJL: TransAction Books.
- Rushton, J. Philippe; Jensen, Arthur R (2005). "Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy and Law 11 (2): 246–8. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235.
- Rushton, J. Philippe; Jensen, Arthur R (2006). "The Totality of Available Evidence Shows the Race IQ Gap Still Remains" (PDF). Psychological Science 16 (10): 921–2. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01803.x. PMID 17100794.
- Rushton, J. Philippe; Jensen, Arthur R (2010). "Race and IQ: A theory-based review of the research in Richard Nisbett's Intelligence and How to Get It" (PDF). The Open Psychology Journal 3: 9–35. doi:10.2174/1874350101003010009.
- Saloojee, Haroon; Pettifor, John M (December 2001). "Iron deficiency and impaired child development". BMJ 323 (7326): 1377–8. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377. PMC 1121846. PMID 11744547.
- Scarr, S.; Weinberg, R. A. (1990). "The nature-nurture problem revisited: The Minnesota adoption studies". Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects 1. pp. 121–151.
- Scarr, S.; Weinberg, R. A. (1976). "IQ test performance of black children adopted by White families". American Psychologist 31: 726–739. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.31.10.726.
- Schacter, Daniel; Gilbert, Daniel; Wegner, Daniel (2007). "Psychology". New York: Worth Publishing. ISBN 0-7167-5215-8.
- Schaefer, Richard T., ed. (2008). "Race". Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity and Society 2. SAGE. p. 1091.
- Serpell, Robert (2000). "Intelligence and Culture". In Sternberg, Robert J. Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. pp. 549–577. ISBN 978-0-521-59648-0.
- Shurkin, Joel (2006). Broken Genius: The Rise and Fall of William Shockley, Creator of the Electronic Age. London: Macmillan. ISBN 978-1-4039-8815-7. Lay summary (2 June 2013).
- Shuttleworth-Edwards, AB; Kemp, RD; Rust, AL; Muirhead, JG; Hartman, NP; Radloff, SE (October 2004). "Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 26 (7): 903–20. doi:10.1080/13803390490510824. PMID 15742541.
- Snyderman, Mark; Rothman, Stanley (1987). "Survey of expert opinion on intelligence and aptitude testing" 42. American Psychologist: 137–44. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.42.2.137.
- Sternberg, Robert J; Grigorenko, Elena L; Kidd, Kenneth K (2005). "Intelligence, Race, and Genetics" (PDF). American Psychologist 60 (1): 46–59. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46. PMID 15641921.
- Sternberg, R. J.; Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). "The Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions in Writing an Article About the Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson (2007)". Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 (2): 221–223. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00040.x.
- Spiro, Jonathan P. (2009). Defending the Master Race: Conservation, Eugenics, and the Legacy of Madison Grant. Univ. of Vermont Press. ISBN 978-1-58465-715-6. Lay summary (29 September 2010).
- Suzuki, Lisa; Aronson, Joshua (2005). "The cultural malleability of intelligence and its impact on the racial/ethnic hierarchy". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 11 (2): 320–327. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.320.
- Tang, Hua; Quertermous, Tom; Rodriguez, Beatriz; Kardia, Sharon L.R.; Zhu, Xiaofeng; Brown, Andrew; Pankow, James S.; Province, Michael A.; Hunt, Steven C.; Boerwinkle, Eric; Schork, Nicholas J.; Risch, Neil J. (February 2005). "Genetic structure, self-identified race/ethnicity, and confounding in case-control association studies". American Journal of Human Genetics 7 (2): 268–75. doi:10.1086/427888. PMC 1196372. PMID 15625622.
- Templeton, A. R. (2001), "The Genetic and Evolutionary Significance of Human Races", in Fish, J. M., Race and Intelligence Separating Science from Myth, London: Routledge, pp. 31–55, ISBN 978-0805837575
- Thompson, Paul M.; Cannon, Tyrone D.; Narr, Katherine L.; van Erp, Theo; Poutanen, Veli-Pekka; Huttunen, Matti; Lönnqvist, Jouko; Standertskjöld-Nordenstam, Carl-Gustaf; Kaprio, Jaakko; Khaledy, Mohammad; Dail, Rajneesh; Zoumalan, Chris I.; Toga, Arthur W. (2001). "Genetic influences on brain structure" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience 4 (12): 1253–58. doi:10.1038/nn758. PMID 11694885.
- Tizard, Barbara; Cooperman, Oliver; Joseph, Anne; Tizard, Jack (June 1972). "Environmental effects on language development: A study of young children in long-stay residential nurseries". Child Development (Blackwell Publishing) 43 (2): 337–58. doi:10.2307/1127540. JSTOR 1127540.
- Tucker, William H (2002). The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund. University of Illinois Press. ISBN 0-252-02762-0.
- Verney, Steven P; Granholn, Eric; Marshall, Sandra P; Malcarne, Vanessa L; Saccuzzo, Dennis P (September 2005). "Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment". Assessment 12 (3): 303–19. doi:10.1177/1073191105276674. PMID 16123251.
- Wade, Nicholas (13 June 2011). "Scientists Measure the Accuracy of a Racism Claim". New York Times.
- Weinberg, Richard A.; Scarr, Sandra; Waldman, Irwin D. (1992). "The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A Follow-Up of IQ Test Performance at Adolescence". Intelligence 16 (1): 117–35. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(92)90028-P.
- Wicherts, Jelte M; Borsboom, Denny; Dolan, Conor V (2010). "Why national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence". Personality and Individual Differences 48: 91–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.028.
- Wicherts, J.M.; Dolan, Conor V.; van der Maas, Han L.J. (2009). "A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans" (PDF). Intelligence (Elsevier) 38 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002. Retrieved 2010-10-02.
- Witelson, SF; Beresh, H; Kigar, DL (2006). "Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factor". Brain (Oxford University Press) 129 (2): 386–98. doi:10.1093/brain/awh696. PMID 16339797.
- Wooldridge, Adrian (1995). "Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England c. 1860-c. 1990". Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-39515-1.
- Wroe, Andrew (2008). The Republican Party and Immigration Politics: from Proposition 187 to George W. Bush. Macmillan. p. 294. ISBN 978-0-230-60053-9.
- Zinkstok, Janneke R; De Wilde, Odette; Van Amelsvoort, Therese AMJ; Tanck, Michael W; Baas, Frank; Linszen, Don H (2007). "Association between the DTNBP1 gene and intelligence: a case-control study in young patients with schizophrenia and related disorders and unaffected siblings". Behavioral and Brain Functions 3: 19. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-3-19. PMC 1864987. PMID 17445278.
External links
- June 2005 issue of Psychology, Public Policy and Law, containing papers arguing various perspectives about race and intelligence.
- Debate between James Flynn and Charles Murray about whether the black/white IQ gap is shrinking or staying the same – November 2006.
- Race and Intelligence: Science's last taboo, a TV documentary made by the UK's Channel 4 public television station, featuring Somali-British journalist Rageh Omaar - October 2009.
|