Serial position effect
Serial position effect is the tendency of a person to recall the first and last items in a series best, and the middle items worst.[1] The term was coined by Hermann Ebbinghaus through studies he performed on himself, and refers to the finding that recall accuracy varies as a function of an item's position within a study list.[2] When asked to recall a list of items in any order (free recall), people tend to begin recall with the end of the list, recalling those items best (the recency effect). Among earlier list items, the first few items are recalled more frequently than the middle items (the primacy effect).[3][4]
One suggested reason for the primacy effect is that the initial items presented are most effectively stored in long-term memory because of the greater amount of processing devoted to them. (The first list item can be rehearsed by itself; the second must be rehearsed along with the first, the third along with the first and second, and so on.) The primacy effect is reduced when items are presented quickly and is enhanced when presented slowly (factors that reduce and enhance processing of each item and thus permanent storage). Longer presentation lists have been found to reduce the primacy effect.[4]
One theorized reason for the recency effect is that these items are still present in working memory when recall is solicited. Items that benefit from neither (the middle items) are recalled most poorly. An additional explanation for the recency effect is related to temporal context: if tested immediately after rehearsal, the current temporal context can serve as a retrieval cue, which would predict more recent items to have a higher likelihood of recall than items that were studied in a different temporal context (earlier in the list).[5] The recency effect is reduced when an interfering task is given. Intervening tasks involve working memory, as the distractor activity, if exceeding 15 to 30 seconds in duration, can cancel out the recency effect.[6] Additionally, if recall comes immediately after test, the recency effect is consistent regardless of the length of the studied list,[4] or presentation rate.[7]
Amnesiacs with poor ability to form permanent long-term memories do not show a primacy effect, but do show a recency effect if recall comes immediately after study.[8] People with Alzheimer's Disease exhibit a reduced primacy effect but do not produce a recency effect in recall.[9]
Primacy effect
The primacy effect, in psychology and sociology, is a cognitive bias that results in a subject recalling primary information presented better than information presented later on. For example, a subject who reads a sufficiently long list of words is more likely to remember words toward the beginning than words in the middle.
Many researchers tried to explain this phenomenon through free recall tests. In some experiments in the late 20th century, it was noted that participants who knew that they were going to be tested on a list presented to them would rehearse items. Meaning, as items were presented, the participants would repeat those items to themselves and as new items were presented, the participants would continue to rehearse previous items along with the newer items. It was demonstrated that the primacy effect had a greater influence on recall when there was more time between presentation of items so that participants would have a greater chance to rehearse previous (prime) items.[10][11][12]
Overt-Rehearsal was a technique that was meant to test participants' rehearsal patterns. In an experiment using this technique, participants were asked to recite out loud the items that come to mind. In this way, the experimenter was able to see that participants would repeat earlier items more than items in the middle of the list, thus rehearsing them more frequently and having a better recall of the prime items than the middle items later on.[13]
In another experiment, by Brodie and Murdock, the recency effect was found to be partially responsible for the primacy effect.[14] In their experiment, they also used the overt-rehearsal technique and found that in addition to rehearsing earlier items more than later items, participants were rehearsing earlier items later on in the list. In this way, earlier items were closer to the test period by way of rehearsal and could be partially explained by the recency effect.
In 2013, a study showed that primacy effect is also prominent in decision making based on experience in a repeated choice paradigm, a learning process also known as operant conditioning. The authors showed that there is a large importance to the value of the first reward on subsequent behaviour, a phenomenon they denoted as outcome primacy.[15]
In another study, the subjects received one of two sentences. For example, one may be given "Steve is smart, diligent, critical, impulsive, and jealous." and the other "Steve is jealous, impulsive, critical, diligent, and smart." These two sentences contain the same information. The first one suggests positive trait at the beginning while the second one has negative traits. Researchers found that the subjects evaluated Steve more positive when given the first sentence, compared to the second one.[16]
Recency effect
Two traditional classes of theories explain the recency effect.
Dual-store models
These models postulate that later study list items are retrieved from a highly accessible short-term buffer, i.e. the short-term store (STS) in human memory. This allows items that are recently studied to have an advantage over those that were studied earlier, as earlier study items have to be retrieved with greater effort from one’s long-term memory store (LTS).
An important prediction of such models is that the presentation of a distractor, for example solving arithmetic problems for 10–30 seconds, during the retention period (the time between list presentation and test) attenuates the recency effect. Since the STS has limited capacity, the distractor displaces later study list items from the STS so that at test, these items can only be retrieved from the LTS, and have lost their earlier advantage of being more easily retrieved from the short-term buffer. As such, dual-store models successfully account for both the recency effect in immediate recall tasks, and the attenuation of such an effect in the delayed free recall task.
A major problem with this model, however, is that it cannot predict the long-term recency effect observed in delayed recall, when a distractor intervenes between each study item during the interstimulus interval (continuous distractor task).[17] Since the distractor is still present after the last study item, it should displace the study item from STS such that the recency effect is attenuated. The existence of this long-term recency effect thus raises the possibility that immediate and long-term recency effects share a common mechanism.[18]
Single-store models
According to single-store theories, a single mechanism is responsible for serial position effects.
A first type of model is based on relative temporal distinctiveness, in which the time lag between test and the study of each list item determines the relative competitiveness of an item’s memory trace at retrieval.[17][19] In this model, end-of-list items are thought to be more distinct, and hence more easily retrieved.
Another type of model is based on contextual variability, which postulates that retrieval of items from memory is cued not only based on one’s mental representation of the study item itself, but also of the study context.[20][21] Since context varies and increasingly changes with time, on an immediate free-recall test, when memory items compete for retrieval, more recently studied items will have more similar encoding contexts to the test context, and are more likely to be recalled.
Outside immediate free recall, these models can also predict the presence or absence of the recency effect in delayed free recall and continual-distractor free-recall conditions. Under delayed recall conditions, the state of test context would have drifted away with an increasing retention interval, leading to an attenuated recency effect. Under continual distractor recall conditions, while the increased interpresentation intervals reduces the similarities between the given study contexts and test context, the relative similarities amongst items remains unchanged. As long as the recall process is competitive, recent items will win out, so a recency effect is observed.
Ratio Rule
Overall, an important empirical observation regarding the recency effect is that it is not the absolute duration of retention intervals (RI, the time between end of study and test period) or of inter-presentation intervals (IPI, the time between different study items) that matters. Rather, the amount of recency is determined by the ratio of RI to IPI (the ratio rule). As a result, as long as this ratio is fixed, recency will be observed regardless of the absolute values of intervals, so that recency can be observed at all time scales, a phenomenon known as time scale invariance. This contradicts dual-store models, which assume that recency depends on the size of STS, and the rule governing the displacement of items in the STS.
Potential explanations either then explain the recency effect as occurring through a single, same mechanism, or re-explain it through a different type of model that postulates two different mechanisms for immediate and long-term recency effects. One such explanation is provided by Davelaar et al. (2005),[22] who argue that there are dissociations between immediate and long-term recency phenomena that cannot be explained by a single-component memory model, and who argues for the existence of a STS that explains immediate recency, and a second mechanism based on contextual drift that explains long-term recency.
Related effects
In 1977, William Crano decided to outline a study to further the previous conclusions on the nature of order effects, in particular those of primacy vs. recency, which were said to be unambiguous and opposed in their predictions. The specifics tested by Crano were:[23]
- Change of meaning hypothesis
- "adjectives presented first on a stimulus list established a set, or expectation, through which the meanings of the later descriptors were modified in an attempt to maintain consistency in the mind of the receiver."
- Inconsistency discounting
- "later descriptions on the stimulus list were discounted if inconsistent with earlier trait adjectives."
- Attention decrement hypothesis
- "earlier adjectives would wield considerably more influence than the later ones, and a primacy effect in the typical impression formation task would be expected to occur ... even when the stimulus list contains traits of a high degree of consistency."
The continuity effect or lag-recency effect predicts that having made a successful recall, the next recall is likely to be a neighboring item in serial position during the study period. The difference between the two items' serial position is referred to as serial position lag. Another factor, called the conditional-response probability, represents the likelihood that a recall of a certain serial position lag was made. A graph of serial position lag versus conditional response probability reveals that the next item recalled minimizes absolute lag, with a higher likelihood for the adjacent item rather than the previous one.
See also
- Clive Wearing
- Free recall
- Henry Molaison
- Law of primacy in persuasion
- Learning curve
- List of memory biases
- List of cognitive biases
- Outcome primacy
- Principles of learning
- Peak-end rule
- Reminiscence bump
Notes
- ↑ Coleman, Andrew (2006). Dictionary of Psychology (Second Edition). Oxford University Press. p. 688.
- ↑ Ebbinghaus, Hermann (1913). On memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Teachers College.
- ↑ Deese and Kaufman (1957) Serial effects in recall of unorganized and sequentially organized verbal material, J Exp Psychol. 1957 Sep;54(3):180-7
- 1 2 3 Murdock, Bennet (1962). "Serial Position Effect of Free Recall". Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (2): 482–488. doi:10.1037/h0045106.
- ↑ Howard, Marc W.; Michael J. Kahana (2002). "A Distributed Representation of Temporal Context". Journal of Mathematical Psychology 46: 269–299. doi:10.1006/jmps.2001.1388.
- ↑ Bjork, Robert A.; William B. Whitten (1974). "Recency-Sensitive Retrieval Processes in Long-Term Free Recall". Cognitive Psychology 6: 173–189. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90009-7.
- ↑ Murdock, Bennet; Janet Metcalf (1978). "Controlled Rehearsal in Single-Trial Free Recall". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 17: 309–324. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(78)90201-3.
- ↑ Carlesimo, Giovanni; G.A. Marfia; A. Loasses; C. Caltagirone (1996). "Recency effect in anterograde amneisa: Evidence for distinct memory stores underlying enhanced retrieval of terminal items in immediate and delayed recall paradigms". Neuropsychologia 34 (3): 177–184. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00100-x.
- ↑ Bayley, Peter J.; David P. Salmon; Mark W. Bondi; Barbara K. Bui; John Olichney; Dean C. Delis; Ronald G. Thomas; Leon J. Thai (March 2000). "Comparison of the serial position effect in very mild Alzheimer's disease, mild Alzheimer's disease, and amnesia associated with electroconvulsive therapy". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 6 (3): 290–298. doi:10.1017/S1355617700633040.
- ↑ Glenberg, A.M; M.M. Bradley, J.A. Stevenson, T.A. KrausM.J. Tkachuk, A.L. Gretz (1980). "A two-process account of long-term serial position effects". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6: 355–369. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.4.355.
- ↑ Marshall, P.H.; P.R. Werder (1972). "The effects of the elimination of rehearsal on primacy and recency". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11: 649–653. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(72)80049-5.
- ↑ Rundus, D. "Maintenance rehearsal and long-term recency". Memory and Cognition (8(3)): 226–230.
- ↑ Rundus, D (1971). "An analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall". Journal of Experimental Psychology 89: 63–77. doi:10.1037/h0031185.
- ↑ Brodie, D.A.; B.B. Murdock. "Effects of presentation time on nominal and functional serial position curves in free recall". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16: 185–200. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(77)80046-7.
- ↑ Shteingart, Hanan; Tal Neiman; Yonatan Loewenstein (2013). "The Role of First Impression in Operant Learning" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (2): 476–488. doi:10.1037/a0029550. PMID 22924882.
- ↑ Asch, S. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,41, 258–290.
- 1 2 Bjork & Whitten (1974). Recency sensitive retrieval processes in long-term free recall, Cognitive Psychology, 6, 173–189.
- ↑ Greene, R. L. (1986). "Sources of recency effects in free recall". Psychological Bulletin 99 (12): 221–228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.99.2.221.
- ↑ Neath, I.; Knoedler, A. J. (1994). "Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition and sentence processing". Journal of Memory and Language 33: 776–795. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1037.
- ↑ Howard, M. W.; Kahana, M. (1999). "Contextual variability and serial position effects in free recall". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 24 (4): 923–941. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923.
- ↑ Howard, M. W.; Kahana, M. J. (2002). "A distributed representation of temporal context". Journal of Mathematical Psychology 46 (3): 269–299. doi:10.1006/jmps.2001.1388.
- ↑ Davelaar, E. K.; Goshen-Gottstein, Y.; Ashkenazi, A.; Haarmann, H. J.; Usher, M. (2005). "The demise of short-term memory revisited: Empirical and computational investigations of recency effects". Psychological Review 112: 3–42. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.1.3.
- ↑ Kohler, Christine. "Order Effects Theory: Primacy versus Recency". Center for Interactive Advertising, The University of Texas at Austin. Archived from the original on 2008-01-06. Retrieved 2007-11-04.
References
- Frensch, P.A. (1994). "Composition during serial learning: a serial position effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20 (2): 423–443. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.2.423.
- Healy, A.F.; Havas, D.A.; Parkour, J.T. (2000). "Comparing serial position effects in semantic and episodic memory using reconstruction of order tasks". Journal of Memory and Language 42: 147–167. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2671.
- Glanzer, M.; Cunitz, A. R. (1966). "Two storage mechanisms in Free Recall". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 5: 351–360. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(66)80044-0.
- Kahana, M. J., Howard, M. W., & Polyn, S. M. (2008). Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory. Psychology.
- Howard, M. W.; Kahana, M. (1999). "Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition 25 (4): 923–941. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923.
Further reading
- Luchins, Abraham S. (1959) Primacy-recency in impression formation
- Liebermann, David A. Learning and memory: An integrative approach. Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth, 2004, ISBN 978-0-534-61974-9.