Memory and aging
One of the key concerns of older adults is the experience of memory loss, especially as it is one of the hallmark symptoms of Alzheimer's disease. However, memory loss is qualitatively different in normal aging from the kind of memory loss associated with a diagnosis of Alzheimer's.[1] Occasional lapses in memory are normal in aging adults and understanding the distinction between normal symptoms and warning signs of Alzheimer’s is critical in maintaining cognitive health.
Mild cognitive impairment
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a condition in which people face memory problems more often than that of the average person their age. These symptoms, however, do not prevent them from carrying out normal activities and are not as severe as the symptoms for Alzheimer's disease. Symptoms often include misplacing items, forgetting events or appointments, and having trouble finding words.[2][3]
According to recent research, MCI is seen as the transitional state between cognitive changes of normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Several studies have indicated that individuals with MCI are at an increased risk for developing AD, ranging from 1% to 25% per year; in one study 24% of MCI patients progressed to AD in 2 years and 20% more over 3 years, whereas another study indicated that the progression of MCI subjects was 55% in 4.5 years.[4][5] Some patients with MCI, however, never progress to AD.[6]
Studies have also indicated patterns that are found in both MCI and AD. Much like patients with Alzheimer's disease, those suffering from mild cognitive impairment have difficulty accurately defining words and using them appropriately in sentences when asked. While MCI patients had a lower performance in this task than the control group, AD patients performed worse overall. The abilities of MCI patients stood out, however, due to the ability to provide examples to make up for their difficulties. AD patients failed to use any compensatory strategies and therefore exhibited the difference in use of episodic memory and executive functioning.[7]
Memory decline in normal aging
Normal aging is associated with a decline in various memory abilities in many cognitive tasks; the phenomenon is known as age-related memory impairment (AMI) or age-associated memory impairment (AAMI). The ability to encode new memories of events or facts and working memory shows decline in both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies.[8] Studies comparing the effects of aging on episodic memory, semantic memory, short-term memory and priming find that episodic memory is especially impaired in normal aging; some types of short-term memory are also impaired.[9] The deficits may be related to impairments seen in the ability to refresh recently processed information.[10]
Source information is one type of episodic memory that suffers with old age; this kind of knowledge includes where and when the person learned the information. Knowing the source and context of information can be extremely important in daily decision-making, so this is one way in which memory decline can affect the lives of the elderly. Therefore, reliance on political stereotypes is one way to use their knowledge about the sources when making judgments, and the use of metacognitive knowledge gains importance.[11] This deficit may be related to declines in the ability to bind information together in memory during encoding and retrieve those associations at a later time.[12][13]
Episodic memory is supported by networks spanning frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes. The interconnections in the lobes are presumed to enable distinct aspects of memory, whereas the effects of gray matter lesions have been extensively studied, less is known about the interconnecting fiber tracts. In aging, degradation of white matter structure has emerged as an important general factor, further focusing attention on the critical white matter connections.
Exercise affects many people young and old. For the young if exercise is introduced it can form a constructive habit that can be instilled throughout adult hood. For the elderly, especially ones that have Alzheimer’s or other diseases that affect the memory. When the brain is introduced to exercise the hippocampus part of the brain can regain in size and improve memory. The hippocampus starts to shrink around the age of 30, thus exercising throughout life can help with not only your physical but your mental state of mind.
In particular, associative learning, which is another type of episodic memory, is vulnerable to the effects of aging, and this has been demonstrated across various study paradigms.[14] This has been explained by the Associative Deficit Hypothesis (ADH), which states that aging is associated with a deficiency in creating and retrieving links between single units of information. This can include knowledge about context, events or items. The ability to bind pieces of information together with their episodic context in a coherent whole has been reduced in the elderly population.[15] Furthermore, the older adults’ performances in free recall involved temporal contiguity to a lesser extent than for younger people, indicating that associations regarding contiguity become weaker with age.[16]
Several reasons have been speculated as to why older adults use less effective encoding and retrieval strategies as they age. The first is the “disuse” view, which states that memory strategies are used less by older adults as they move further away from the educational system. Second is the “diminished attentional capacity” hypothesis, which means that older people engage less in self-initiated encoding due to reduced attentional capacity. The third reason is the “memory self-efficacy,” which indicates that older people do not have confidence in their own memory performances, leading to poor consequences.[14] It is known that patients with Alzheimer’s disease and patients with semantic dementia both exhibit difficulty in tasks that involve picture naming and category fluency. This is tied to damage to their semantic network, which stores knowledge of meanings and understandings.
One phenomenon, known as "Senior Moments", is a memory deficit that appears to have a biological cause. When an older adult is interrupted while completing a task, it is likely that the original task at hand can be forgotten. Studies have shown that the brain of an older adult does not have the ability to re-engage after an interruption and continues to focus on the particular interruption unlike that of a younger brain.[17] This inability to multi-task is normal with aging and is expected to become more apparent with the increase of older generations remaining in the work field.
A biological explanation for memory deficits in aging includes a postmortem examination of five brains of elderly people with better memory than average. These people are called the "super aged,” and it was found that these individuals had fewer fiber-like tangles of tau protein than in typical elderly brains. However, a similar amount of amyloid plaque was found.[18]
More recent research has extended established findings of age related decline in executive functioning,[19][20] by examining related cognitive processes that underlie healthy older adults’ sequential performance. Sequential performance refers to the execution of a series steps needed to complete a routine, such as the steps required to make a cup of coffee or drive a car. An important part of healthy aging involves older adults’ use of memory and inhibitory processes to carry out daily activities in a fixed order without forgetting the sequence of steps that were just completed while remembering the next step in the sequence. A recent study[21] examined how young and older adults differ in the underlying representation of a sequence of tasks and their efficiency at retrieving the information needed to complete their routine. Findings from this study revealed that when older and young adults had to remember a sequence of 8 animal images arranged in a fixed order, both age groups spontaneously used the organizational strategy of chunking to facilitate retrieval of information. However, older adults were slower at accessing each chunk compared to younger adults, and were better able to benefit from the use of memory aids, such as verbal rehearsal to remember the order of the fixed sequence. Results from this study suggest that there are age differences in memory and inhibitory processes that affect people’s sequence of actions and the use of memory aids could facilitate the retrieval of information in older age.
Possible causes of memory decline
People commonly associate memory lapses in their mid-30s, 40s, or 50s as a sign of Alzheimer’s disease as they approach later adulthood, but typically this is not the case. Memory lapses can be both aggravating and frustrating but they are due to the overwhelming amount of information that is being taken in by the brain. Issues in memory can also be linked to several common physical and psychological causes, such as: anxiety, dehydration, depression, infections, medication side effects, poor nutrition, vitamin B12 deficiency, psychological stress, substance abuse, chronic alcoholism, thyroid imbalances, and blood clots in the brain. Taking care of your body and mind with appropriate medication, doctoral check-ups, and daily mental and physical exercise can prevent some of these memory issues.[22]
Some memory issues are due to stress, anxiety, or depression. A traumatic life event, such as the death of a spouse, can lead to changes in lifestyle and can leave an elderly person feeling unsure of themselves, sad, and lonely. Dealing with such drastic life changes can therefore leave some people confused or forgetful. While in some cases these feelings may fade, it is important to take these emotional problems seriously. By emotionally supporting a struggling relative and seeking help from a doctor or counselor, the forgetfulness can be improved.[3]
Prevention and treatment
Various actions have been suggested to prevent memory loss or even improve memory.
The Mayo Clinic has suggested seven steps: stay mentally active, socialize regularly, get organized, eat a healthy diet, include physical activity in your daily routine, and manage chronic conditions.[23] Because some of the causes of memory loss include medications, stress, depression, heart disease, alcohol abuse, thyroid problems, vitamin B12 deficiency, not drinking enough water, and not eating nutritiously, fixing those problems could be a simple, effective way to slow down dementia. Some say that exercise is the best way to prevent memory problems, because that would increase blood flow to the brain and perhaps help new brain cells grow. A healthy diet is also critical, partly because it has been demonstrated that healthy eaters are much less likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease.[24]
The treatment will depend on the cause of memory loss, but various drugs to treat Alzheimer’s disease have been suggested in recent years. There are four drugs currently approved by the FDA for the treatment of Alzheimer’s, and they all act on the cholinergic system: Donepezil (Aricept), Galantamine (Reminyl), Rivastigmine (Exelon), and Tacrine (Cognex). Although these medications are not the cure for Alzheimer’s, symptoms may be reduced for up to eighteen months for mild or moderate dementia. These drugs do not forestall the ultimate decline to full Alzheimer's.[25]
Also, modality is important in determining the strength of the memory. For instance, auditory creates stronger memory abilities than visual. This is shown by the higher recency and primacy effects of an auditory recall test compared to that of a visual test. Research has shown that auditory training, through instrumental musical activity or practice, can help preserve memory abilities as one ages. Specifically, in Hanna-Pladdy and McKay's experiment, they tested and found that the number of years of musical training, all things equal, leads to a better performance in non-verbal memory and increases the life span on cognition abilities in one's advanced years.[26]
The use of memory aids is helpful in fighting cognitive signs of aging. Keeping a "to do" list will help assure that certain tasks are completed and not forgotten. Establishing day-to-day routines will make everyday tasks, such as taking medication, easier to remember if they occur at the same time every day. Putting everything in its rightful place will help to avoid confusion. Keeping important items in a place where they can always be seen will save you time when they're needed. Using simple associations to remember names, events, or objects can make recalling things much easier. Finally, keeping a calendar to display important dates and times will make remembering much easier.
The easiest way to prevent memory decline in elderhood is to stay active throughout your 40s and 50s. Being mentally active and learning new skills during middle adulthood and as you age is speculated to lower the risk of Alzheimer's disease. By performing new tasks and learning new skills, the brain is forced to focus more than it would on a task in which you have already mastered. In essence, acquiring new skills is a way to exercise your brain. Those who enjoy learning and want to stay mentally active can participate in a new program hosted at Columbia University, as well as 117 other colleges and universities across the country. This program is focused on the elderly and allows them to take classes in academic courses as well as courses involving exercising and skills without the worry of reading, papers, homework, and grades.[27]
New research has identified specific impaired neurons within the human brain that when stimulated, can be recovered. Through studies with fruit flies, it was discovered by Professor Ron Davis that, "...once the appropriate neurons are identified in people, in principle at least, one could potentially develop drugs to hit those neurons and rescue those memories affected by the aging process." Research such as this, is bringing us one step closer to developing preventative methods and drugs that can reverse and slow the cognitive declines associated with aging.[28]
Scientists and nutritionists have linked the consumption of certain foods with improved memory and the prevention of Alzheimer's and age-related memory decline.
In her 2010 publication on Alzheimer's, Jean Carper suggests many food practices to prevent the onset of this disease. She encourages individuals to eat antioxidant-rich foods, choline-rich foods, curry, fatty fish, folic acid, nuts, and vinegar; to drink juices, coffee, tea, and red wine (while practicing moderation with alcohol in general); to avoid sugar; and to follow a low-glycemic index diet and/or Mediterranean diet.[29][30]
A 2011 study suggests that increasing Vitamin C and Vitamin E in diets can increase verbal memory functions. Vitamin C is an antioxidant that can protect brain tissues from inflammation and oxidation damage. Foods rich in Vitamin C include broccoli, Brussels sprouts, cabbage, cantaloupe, cauliflower, grapefruit, green and red peppers, kale, kiwi, mango, oranges, papaya, pineapple, strawberries, and tomato juice. Vitamin E can protect brain cell membranes from free radicals. Foods rich in Vitamin E include almonds, canola oil, grape seed oil, hazelnuts, papaya, safflower oil, sunflower seeds, and wheat germ oil. Vitamin E is also prevalent in leafy greens, including arugula, beet greens, collard greens, kale, mustard greens, rapini, spinach, and Swiss chard. There are conflicting opinions as to whether such vitamins must be consumed in foods or if dietary supplements are also effective.[31][32]
2011 and 2012 studies suggest that folic acid and Vitamin B12 may also help prevent memory decline. Foods rich in Vitamin B12 include beef liver, clams, dairy products, fish, poultry, and meat.[33][34][35]
Diets high in saturated fats and refined sugars have been associated with increased Alzheimer's risk, while low saturated fat and low-glycemic index diets have been associated with decreased Alzheimer's risk. Lean meats, poultry, and low-fat dairy products characterize a low-saturated-fat diet, as well as avoidance of butter in favor of unsaturated fats, such as canola oil, grape seed oil, olive oil, and sunflower oil. Foods with a low-glycemic index include apples, bran cereal, brown rice, citrus fruits, grain breads with seeds, grapes, legumes, milk, nuts, pasta, pears, steel-cut and large-flake oats, sweet potatoes, wild rice, and yogurt. Omega-3 fatty acids found in oily fish, such as herring, mackerel, salmon, sardines, and trout, can also improve memory functions.[36][37]
Evidence has accumulated that compounds called flavanols naturally occurring in cocoa beans can enhance brain blood flow and improve cognitive health.[38] In 2014, a publication by a group of scientists from seven American institutes reported a controlled randomized trial applying a high-resolution variant of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of the brains of 37 healthy, but sedentary, volunteers aged 50–69, who were daily given over a three-month period a specially-prepared cocoa drink.[39][40] Each subject was randomly chosen to always receive a drink containing either a large dose of flavanols contained in cocoa solids — 900 mg — or a low dose, 10 mg. Of primary interest in the fMRI images was blood volume in the dentate gyrus (DG), a region in the hippocampal formation whose function declines with human aging and is therefore considered to be a possible crux of age-related memory decline. Previous studies had shown that flavanol consumption can increase dendritic spine and capillary density in the DG of adult mice.[41] The image data of humans revealed a strong correlation between flavanol dose and enhancement of DG blood volume over this three-month study. The subjects were also challenged before and after the period with a novel object recognition task, selection of which was guided by a previous study that had established that this type of task is a cognitive feature of age-related hippocampal dysfunction that localizes to the DG. The subjects were shown a series of mathematically generated images that could be varied in a quantitative way, and asked to identify previously unseen patterns, either immediately in sequential pairs or after 41 images had been seen. It was found that the high dose of flavanol resulted in significantly better performance in terms of response time to reject previously unseen patterns. The same test applied to 35 young healthy subjects had shown that their performance correlated with higher DG blood volume in their fMRI images. While cautioning that this small study needs to be confirmed with more subjects, one of the authors summarized the overall result by saying “If a participant had the memory of a typical 60-year-old at the beginning of the study, after three months that person on average had the memory of a typical 30- or 40-year-old”, and that "flavanols improves the function of humans’ dentate gyrus, particularly in aging humans”. It was also emphasized that the dose of flavanols used in this study is far larger than can be obtained from typical commercial cocoa or chocolate, as the processing typically eliminates most of the natural flavanols.
Caregiving and daily life
Caring for an adult with memory issues can be a day to day struggle. Every task can become an extreme chore if not approached and handled correctly. By keeping the patient active, focusing on their positive abilities, and avoiding stress, these tasks can easily be accomplished. Routines for bathing and dressing must be organized in a way so that the patient still feels a sense of independence. Simple approaches such as finding clothes with large buttons, elastic waist bands, or Velcro straps can ease the struggles of getting dressed in the morning. Further, finances must be managed. Changing passwords to prevent over-use and involving a trusted family member or friend in managing accounts can prevent financial issues. When household chores begin to pile up, find ways to break down large tasks into small, manageable steps that can be rewarded. Finally, talking with and visiting a family member or friend with memory issues is very important. Using a respectful and simple approach, talking one-on-one can ease the pain of social isolation and bring much mental stimulation.[42]
The most important thing to remember when caring for an adult with memory issues is to give them independence in a respectful manner. Finding tasks for them, keeping their mind busy, and rewarding them for a job well done will promote a desire for mentally stimulating activities.
Domains of memory mostly spared
In contrast, implicit, or procedural memory, typically shows no decline with age.[43] Other types of short-term memory show little decline,[9] and semantic knowledge (e.g. vocabulary) actually improves with age.[44] In addition, the enhancement seen in memory for emotional events is also maintained with age.[45]
Recently (2010), experiments that have tested for the significance of under-performance of memory for an older adult group as compared to a young adult group, hypothesized that the deficit in associate memory due to age can be linked with a physical deficit. This deficit can be explained by the inefficient processing in the medial-temporal regions. This region is important in episodic memory, which is one of the two types of long-term human memory, and it contains the hippocampi, which are crucial in creating memorial association between items.[46]
Losing working memory has been cited as being the primary reason for a decline in a variety of cognitive tasks due to aging. These tasks include long-term memory, problem solving, decision making, and language.[47] Working memory involves the manipulation of information that is being obtained, and then using this information to complete a task. For example, the ability of one to recite numbers they have just been given backwards requires working memory, rather than just simple rehearsal of the numbers which would require only short-term memory. One's ability to tap into one's working memory declines as the aging process progresses.[47] It has been seen that the more complex a task is, the more difficulty the aging person has with completing this task. Active reorganization and manipulation of information becomes increasingly harder as adults age.[48] When an older individual is completing a task, such as having a conversation or doing work, they are using their working memory to help them complete this task. As they age, their ability to multi-task seems to decline; thus after an interruption it is often more difficult for an aging individual to successfully finish the task at hand.[49] Additionally, working memory plays a role in the comprehension and production of speech. There is often a decline in sentence comprehension and sentence production as individuals age. Rather than linking this decline directly to deficits in linguistic ability, it is actually deficits in working memory that contribute to these decreasing language skills.[50]
Theories about memory and aging
Tests and data show that as people age, the contiguity effect weakens. This is supported by the associative deficit theory of memory, which asserts old people's poor memory performance is attributed to their difficulty in creating and retaining cohesive episodes. The supporting research in this test, after controlling for sex, education, and other health-related issues, show that greater age was associated with lower hit and greater false alarm rates, and also a more liberal bias response on recognition tests.[51]
Older people have a higher tendency to make outside intrusions during a memory test. This can be attributed to the inhibition effect. Inhibition caused participants to take longer time in recalling or recognizing an item, and also subjected the participants to make more frequent errors. For instance, in a study using metaphors as the test subject, older participants rejected correct metaphors more often than literally false statements.[52]
Working memory, which as previously stated is a memory system that stores and manipulates information as we complete cognitive tasks, demonstrates great declines during the aging process. There have been various theories offered to explain why these changes may occur,which include fewer attentional resources, slower speed of processing, less capacity to hold information, and lack of inhibitory control. All of these theories offer strong arguments,and it is likely that the decline in working memory is due to the problems cited in all of these areas.
Some theorists argue that the capacity of working memory decreases as we age, and we are able to hold less information.[53] In this theory, declines in working memory are described as the result of limiting the amount of information an individual can simultaneously keep active, so that a higher degree of integration and manipulation of information is not possible because the products of earlier memory processing are forgotten before the subsequent products.[54]
Another theory that is being examined to explain age related declines in working memory is that there is a limit in attentional resources seen as we age. This means that older individuals are less capable of dividing their attention between two tasks, and thus tasks with higher attentional demands are more difficult to complete due to a reduction in mental energy.[47] Tasks that are simple and more automatic, however, see fewer declines as we age. Working memory tasks often involve divided attention, thus they are more likely to strain the limited resources of aging individuals.[47]
Speed of processing is another theory that has been raised to explain working memory deficits. As a result of various studies he has completed examining this topic, Salthouse argues that as we age our speed of processing information decreases significantly. It is this decrease in processing speed that is then responsible for our inability to use working memory efficiently as we age.[47] The younger persons brain is able to obtain and process information at a quicker rate which allows for subsequent integration and manipulation needed to complete the cognitive task at hand. As this processing slows, cognitive tasks that rely on quick processing speed then become more difficult.[47]
Finally, the theory of inhibitory control has been offered to account for decline seen in working memory. This theory examines the idea that older adults are unable to suppress irrelevant information in working memory, and thus the capacity for relevant information is subsequently limited. Less space for new stimuli due may attribute to the declines seen in an individual's working memory as they age.[47]
As we age, deficits are seen in the ability to integrate, manipulate, and reorganize the contents of working memory in order to complete higher level cognitive tasks such as problem solving, decision making, goal setting, and planning. More research must be completed in order to determine what the exact cause of these age-related deficits in working memory are. It is likely that attention, processing speed, capacity reduction, and inhibitory control may all play a role in these age-related deficits. The brain regions that are active during working memory tasks are also being evaluated, and research has shown that different parts of the brain are activated during working memory in younger adults as compared to older adults. This suggests that younger and older adults are performing these tasks differently.[47]
Qualitative changes
Most research on memory and aging has focused on how older adults perform worse at a particular memory task. However, researchers have also discovered that simply saying that older adults are doing the same thing, only less of it, is not always accurate. In some cases, older adults seem to be using different strategies than younger adults. For example, brain imaging studies have revealed that older adults are more likely to use both hemispheres when completing memory tasks than younger adults.[55] In addition, older adults sometimes show a positivity effect when remembering information, which seems to be a result of the increased focus on regulating emotion seen with age.[45] For instance, eye tracking reveals that older adults showed preferential looking toward happy faces and away from sad faces.[56]
See also
- Aging
- Aging brain
- Aging movement control
- Brain Fart
- Childhood amnesia
- Cognitive enhancement
- Life extension
- List of life extension related topics
- Memory
- Old age
- Testamentary capacity
References
- ↑ Budson AE, Price BH (February 2005). "Memory dysfunction" (PDF). N. Engl. J. Med. 352 (7): 692–9. doi:10.1056/NEJMra041071. PMID 15716563.
- ↑ Mufson, EJ.; Binder, L.; Counts, SE.; DeKosky, ST.; de Toledo-Morrell, L.; Ginsberg, SD.; Ikonomovic, MD.; et, al (Jan 2012). "Mild cognitive impairment: pathology and mechanisms.". Acta Neuropathol 123 (1): 13–30. doi:10.1007/s00401-011-0884-1. PMC 3282485. PMID 22101321.
- 1 2 "Forgetfulness: Knowing When To Ask For Help | National Institute on Aging". Nia.nih.gov. 2012-04-18. Retrieved 2012-08-17.
- ↑ Arnáiz E, Almkvist O (2003). "Neuropsychological features of mild cognitive impairment and preclinical Alzheimer's disease". Acta Neurol. Scand., Suppl. 179: 34–41. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0404.107.s179.7.x. PMID 12603249.
- ↑ Portet, F.; Ousset, PJ.; Visser, PJ.; Frisoni, GB.; Nobili, F.; Scheltens, P.; Vellas, B.; Touchon, J. (Jun 2006). "Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: a critical review of the concept and new diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer's Disease.". J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77 (6): 714–8. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.085332. PMC 2077456. PMID 16549412.
- ↑ Jak, AJ.; Bangen, KJ.; Wierenga, CE.; Delano-Wood, L.; Corey-Bloom, J.; Bondi, MW. (2009). "Contributions of neuropsychology and neuroimaging to understanding clinical subtypes of mild cognitive impairment.". Int Rev Neurobiol 84: 81–103. doi:10.1016/S0074-7742(09)00405-X. PMC 2864107. PMID 19501714.
- ↑ "Millennium Web Catalog". Journal of Neurolinguistics 25: 263–275. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2012.02.002. Retrieved 2012-08-17.
- ↑ Hedden T, Gabrieli JD (February 2004). "Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5 (2): 87–96. doi:10.1038/nrn1323. PMID 14735112.
- 1 2 Nilsson LG (2003). "Memory function in normal aging". Acta Neurol. Scand., Suppl. 179: 7–13. PMID 12603244.
- ↑ Johnson MK, Reeder JA, Raye CL, Mitchell KJ (2002). "Second thoughts versus second looks: An age-related deficit in selectively refreshing just-active information" (PDF). Psychological Science 13 (1): 64–7. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00411. PMID 11892780.
- ↑ Kuhlmann, Beatrice. "Older Adults' Use of Metacognitive Knowledge in Source Monitoring: Spared Monitoring but Impaired Control" (PDF). Psychology and Aging. American Psychological Association. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ Naveh-Benjamin M (September 2000). "Adult age differences in memory performance: tests of an associative deficit hypothesis". J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 26 (5): 1170–87. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1170. PMID 11009251.
- ↑ Mitchell KJ, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Mather M, D'Esposito M (September 2000). "Aging and reflective processes of working memory: binding and test load deficits". Psychol Aging 15 (3): 527–41. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.527. PMID 11014715.
- 1 2 Light, Leah. "Memory and Aging: Four Hypotheses in Search of Data". Annu. Rev. Psychol. Annual Reviews Inc. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ Provyn, Jennifer. "Effects of Age on Contextually Mediated Associations in Paired Associate Learnin". Psychology and Aging. American Psychological Association. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ Kahana, Michael. "Age Dissociates Recency and Lag Recency Effects in Free Recall" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology. American Psychological Association. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ "Memory Study Explains 'Senior Moments' | Working Memory & Aging | Multitasking Abilities". LiveScience. 2011-04-11. Retrieved 2012-08-17.
- ↑ EurekAlert. 'Super-aged' brains reveal first secrets of sharp memory in old age.
- ↑ Verhaeghen, P; Cerella, J (2002). "Aging, executive control, and attention: A review of met-analyses". Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews 26: 849–857. doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00071-4.
- ↑ West, R (1996). "An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging". Psychological Bulletin 120: 272–292. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272. PMID 8831298.
- ↑ Li, K. Z. H., Blair, M. A., & Chow, V. S. M. "Sequential performance in young and older adults: Evidence of chunking and inhibition." (PDF). Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 17: 270–295. doi:10.1080/13825580903165428.
- ↑ http://www.apa.org/pi/aging/memory-and-aging.pdf
- ↑ Mayo Clinic Staff. "Memory loss: 7 tips to improve your memory". Mayo Clinic. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ Taylor, Liz (5 February 2007). "Fighting memory loss through brain games, exercise and rest". The Seattle Times (The Seattle Times Company). Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ "Improving Memory and Treating Memory Loss". Harvard Medical School. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ Hanna-Pladdy, Brenda; MacKay, Alicia (May 2011). "The relation between instrumental musical activity and cognitive aging". Neuropsychology 25 (3): 378–386. doi:10.1037/a0021895. PMID 21463047.
- ↑ Grady, Denise (7 March 2012). "Retirees Are Using Education to Exercise an Aging Brain". The New York Times.
- ↑ Thornhill, Ted (4 April 2012). "Age-related memory loss may be reversible, say scientists who've pinpointed the neurons responsible for it". Daily Mail (London).
- ↑ Carper, Jean (2010). 100 Simple Things You Can Do to Prevent Alzheimer’s. New York: Hachette Book Group. ISBN 9780316086851. OCLC 548642154.
- ↑ English, Kirsty (March 12, 2012). "Eat Curry, Have a Cuppa, Sleep Well and Avoid Alzheimer’s.". The Mirror.
- ↑ Emmanuelle, Kesse-Guyot; Fezeu Leopold; Jeandel Claude; et al. (September 2011). "French adults' cognitive performance after daily supplementation with antioxidant vitamins and minerals at nutritional doses: a post hoc analysis of the Supplementation in Vitamins and Mineral Antioxidants". American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 94 (3): 829–899.
- ↑ Beck, Leslie (October 5, 2011). "Wednesday "Hone your memory with vitamin C-rich fruit"". The Globe.
- ↑ Walker, Janine G.; Philip J. Batterham; et al. (January 2012). "Oral folic acid and vitamin B-12 supplementation to prevent cognitive decline in community-dwelling older adults with depressive symptoms—the Beyond Ageing Project: a randomized controlled trial". American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 95 (1): 194–203. doi:10.3945/ajcn.110.007799.
- ↑ Tangney, C. C.; N.T. Aggarwal; et al. (September 2011). "Vitamin B12, cognition, and brain MRI measures A cross-sectional examination". Neurology 77 (13): 1276–1282. doi:10.1212/wnl.0b013e3182315a33.
- ↑ Searing, Linda (October 10, 2011). "Insufficient Vitamin B12 may be linked to memory problems in older people". Washington Post.
- ↑ Bayer-Carter, Jennifer L.; Pattie S. Green; et al. (2011). "Diet Intervention and Cerebrospinal Fluid Biomarkers in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment". Archives of Neurology 68 (6): 743–752. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2011.125.
- ↑ Beck, Leslie (June 15, 2011). "Cut saturated fats, refined sugars to boost your brain". The Globe.
- ↑ "Flavanols in cocoa may offer benefits to the brain". International Journal of Medical Sciences. Nov 2, 2007. Retrieved Nov 1, 2014.
- ↑ Ingham, Richard; Agence France-Presse (5 February 2007). "Cocoa clue to reversing age-related memory loss". Japan Times. Retrieved 9 May 2011.
- ↑ Brickman, Adam M.; with eight others (Oct 26, 2014). "Enhancing dentate gyrus function with dietary flavanols improves cognition in older adults". Nature Neuroscience 17: 1798–1803. doi:10.1038/nn.3850.
- ↑ van Praag, Henriette; with ten others (2007). "Plant-Derived Flavanol (−)Epicatechin Enhances Angiogenesis and Retention of Spatial Memory in Mice". Journal of Neuroscience 27: 5869–5878. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0914-07.2007.
- ↑ "Practical Tips for Daily Life | UCSF Memory and Aging Center". Memory.ucsf.edu. Retrieved 2012-08-17.
- ↑ Fleischman DA, Wilson RS, Gabrieli JD, Bienias JL, Bennett DA (December 2004). "A longitudinal study of implicit and explicit memory in old persons". Psychol Aging 19 (4): 617–25. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.617. PMID 15584787.
- ↑ Verhaeghen P (June 2003). "Aging and vocabulary scores: a meta-analysis". Psychol Aging 18 (2): 332–9. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.332. PMID 12825780.
- 1 2 Mather M, Carstensen LL (October 2005). "Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory" (PDF). Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 9 (10): 496–502. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005. PMID 16154382.
- ↑ Craik, Fergus I. M.; Luo, Lin; Sakuta, Yuiko (Dec 2010). "Effects of aging and divided attention on memory for items and their contexts". Psychology and Aging 25 (4): 968–979. doi:10.1037/a0020276. PMID 20973605.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Glisky, El (2007). Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. Boca Raton, F: CRC Press.
- ↑ Suprenant, A.; Bireta T.; Farley L. "A brief history of memory and aging." (PDF). The Foundations of Remembering (7): 108–120. Retrieved April 20, 2012.
- ↑ Ritchel, M (April 11, 2011). "Multitasking takes toll on memory, study finds". New York Times. Retrieved April 20, 2012.
- ↑ Thorton, R.; Light L. "Language compensation and production in normal aging" (PDF). Handbook of Psychology and Aging 6: 1–16. Retrieved April 20, 2012.
- ↑ Bender, AR; Naveh-Benjamin, M; Raz, N (Dec 2010). "Associative Deficit in Recognition Memory in a Lifespan Sample of Healthy Adults". Psycholy Aging 25 (4): 940–948. doi:10.1037/a0020595. PMC 3011045. PMID 20822256.
- ↑ Morrone, Isabella; Declercq, Christelle (Sep 2010). "Aging and inhibition processes: The case of metaphor treatment". Psychology and Aging 25 (3): 697–701. doi:10.1037/a0019578. PMID 20853972.
- ↑ Abrams, L; Farrel, M. "Language processing in normal aging" (PDF). Running Head: Language in Aging. Retrieved April 20, 2012.
- ↑ Levitt, Terry (2006). "Processing speed, attentional capacity, and age-related memory change." (PDF). Experimental Aging Research 32: 263–295. doi:10.1080/03610730600699118. Retrieved April 20, 2012.
- ↑ Cabeza R (March 2002). "Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model" (PDF). Psychol Aging 17 (1): 85–100. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85. PMID 11931290.
- ↑ Isaacowitz DM, Wadlinger HA, Goren D, Wilson HR (March 2006). "Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study". Psychol Aging 21 (1): 40–8. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40. PMID 16594790.
External links
- A E Budson and B H Price - Memory dysfunction in neurological practice
- Henry JD, MacLeod MS, Phillips LH, Crawford JR (March 2004). "A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging". Psychol Aging 19 (1): 27–39. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27. PMID 15065929.
- Johnson, MK.; Hashtroudi, S.; Lindsay, DS. (Jul 1993). "Source monitoring.". Psychol Bull 114 (1): 3–28. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3. PMID 8346328.
- Maylor, E.A. (1995). "Prospective memory in normal ageing and dementia". MRC CBU, Cambridge » Bibliography.
- National Institutes of Health - Memory related resources
- Nilsson, LG. (2003). "Memory function in normal aging.". Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 179: 7–13. PMID 12603244.