Research on meditation

EEG technology has been used for meditation research

Since the 1950s hundreds of studies on meditation have been conducted, though many of the early studies were flawed and thus yielded unreliable results.[1][2] More recent reviews have pointed out many of these flaws with the hope of guiding current research into a more fruitful path.[3]

Research on the processes and effects of meditation is a growing subfield of neurological research.[4][5] Modern scientific techniques and instruments, such as fMRI and EEG, have been used to see what happens in the bodies of people when they meditate, and how their bodies and brains change after meditating regularly.[4][6][7]

Meditation is a broad term which encompasses a number of practices.

Mindfulness

Main article: Mindfulness

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses

According to a 2015 systematic review and meta-analysis of systematic reviews of RCTs, evidence supports the use of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) programs to alleviate symptoms of a variety of mental and physical disorders.[8] This review included a combined total of 8683 participants consisting of different patient categories as well as healthy adults and children.[8] A previous study commissioned by the US Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality found that meditation interventions reduce multiple negative dimensions of psychological stress.[9] However, this study used a highly heterogeneous group of meditation styles and many of the studies included in this review were short term studies. Other systematic reviews and meta-analysis show that mindfulness meditation has several mental health benefits such as bringing about reductions in depression symptoms,[10][11][12] and mindfulness interventions also appear to be a promising intervention for managing depression in youth.[13][14] Mindfulness meditation is useful for managing stress[11][15][16] anxiety,[10][11][16] and also appears to be effective in treating substance use disorders.[17][18][19] Other review studies have shown that mindfulness meditation can enhance the psychological functioning of breast cancer survivors,[20] effective for eating disorders,[21][22] and may also be effective in treating psychosis.[23][24][25]

Singular studies

Studies have also shown that rumination and worry contribute to mental illnesses such as depression and anxiety,[26] and mindfulness-based interventions are effective in the reduction of worry.[26][27]

Mindfulness meditation also appears to bring about favorable structural changes in the brain.[4][6][7] One recent study found a significant cortical thickness increase in individuals who underwent a brief -8 weeks- MBSR training program and that this increase was coupled with a significant reduction of several psychological indices related to worry, state anxiety, depression.[28] Another study describes how mindfulness based interventions target neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface.[18]

Some studies suggest that mindfulness meditation contributes to a more coherent and healthy sense of self and identity, when considering aspects such as sense of responsibility, authenticity, compassion, self-acceptance and character.[29][30]

Mindfulness scales

In the relatively new field of western psychological mindfulness, researchers attempt to define and measure the results of mindfulness primarily through controlled, randomised studies of mindfulness intervention on various dependent variables. The participants in mindfulness interventions measure many of the outcomes of such interventions subjectively. For this reason, several mindfulness inventories or scales (a set of questions posed to a subject whose answers output the subject's aggregate answers in the form of a rating or category) have arisen. Twelve such methods are mentioned by the Mindfulness Research Guide[31] Examples include:

Through the use of these scales - which can illuminate self-reported changes in levels of mindfulness, the measurement of other correlated inventories in fields such as subjective well-being, and the measurement of other correlated variables such as health and performance - researchers have produced studies that investigate the nature and effects of mindfulness. The research on the outcomes of mindfulness falls into two main categories: stress reduction and positive-state elevation.

Brain mechanisms

In 2011, National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH) released findings from a study in which magnetic resonance images were taken of the brains of 16 participants 2 weeks before and after the participants joined the mindfulness meditation (MM) program by researchers from Massachusetts General Hospital, Bender Institute of Neuroimaging in Germany, and the University of Massachusetts Medical School. Researchers concluded that

..these findings may represent an underlying brain mechanism associated with mindfulness-based improvements in mental health.[33]

The analgesic effect of MM involves multiple brain mechanisms including the activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.[34] In addition, brief periods of MM training increases the amount of grey matter in the hippocampus and parietal lobe.[35] Other neural changes resulting from MM may increase the efficiency of attentional control.[36]

Participation in MBSR programmes has been found to correlate with decreases in right basolateral amygdala gray matter density,[37] and increases in gray matter concentration within the left hippocampus.[38]

Attention and Mindfulness

Attention networks and mindfulness meditation

Psychological and Buddhists conceptualisations of mindfulness both highlight awareness and attention training as key components, in which levels of mindfulness can be cultivated with practise of mindfulness meditation.[39] Focused attention meditation and open monitoring meditation are distinct types of mindfulness meditation, and the former relates to directing and maintaining attention on a chosen object (e.g. the breath).[40] Open monitoring meditation does not involve focus on a specific object, and instead awareness is grounded in the perceptual features of one’s environment.

Focused attention meditation is typically practiced first to increase the ability to enhance attentional stability, and awareness of mental states with the goal being to transition to open monitoring meditation practise that emphases the ability to monitor moment by moment changes in experience, without a focus of attention to maintain. Mindfulness meditation may lead to greater cognitive flexibility [41][42]

Neurological processes underlying focused attention meditation

It is considered that focused attention meditation entails the activation of attention networks in the following manner:[43]

  1. Initially the alerting network of attention is activated involving sustaining attention on a chosen object. It is associated with the right parietal cortex, right frontal cortex and the thalamus [43]
  2. When mind wandering occurs this relates to activation of the default mode network associated with the following brain areas: the posterior cingulate cortex, posterior lateral parietal/temporal cortices, the cingulate cortex, and the parahippocampal gyrus.[44] This attention network is associated with certain attentional states as introspective thought and daydreaming. Conversely, when de-activated it relates to task-engagement.[45]
  3. Distraction from the focus of attention is detected by the salience network, and is associated with the task of monitoring the focus of attention. The associated brain regions include the cingulate cortex and the anterior insula [46]
  4. When a distracting thought grabs the focus of attention away from the chosen object, the executive function network is capable of inhibiting this from being further processed. This network reduces the distractibility of aspects of one’s environment, and is associated with the basal ganglia, lateral ventral cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) [47]
  5. The orienting network of attention, which controls stimulus selection, involves shifting the focus of attention back to the original object. The superior colliculus and frontal eye fields as well as the temporal parietal junction and the superior parietal cortex are implicated.[48]

Evidence for improvements in three areas of attention

Sustained attention Tasks of sustained attention relate to vigilance and the preparedness that aids completing a particular task goal. Psychological research into the relationship between mindfulness meditation and the sustained attention network have revealed the following:

Selective attention

Executive control attention Executive control attention include functions of inhibiting the conscious processing of distracting information. In the context of mindful meditation, distracting information would relate to attention grabbing mental events such as thoughts related to the future or past.[40]

Emotion regulation and Mindfulness

Approaching emotions in an adaptive way relates to mindful emotion regulation, which aims to decrease avoidance or suppression of emotions, as well as decreasing over-arousal in emotional reactivity in response to events.[60] It is highlighted that emotion regulation is vital to mental stability.[61] Over-involvement with emotions may lead to critical over-analysis of thoughts and emotions, characterising rumination, predictive of poor mental health.[62] Reductions in rumination have been found following Mindfulness meditation practise.[63][64] Under-involvement with addressing difficult emotions -termed avoidance behaviours- also can be problematic [61] as these can bring about maladaptive defences such as denial, suppression, cognitive distortions, development of psychoses, and even substance abuse or self-harm as methods of avoidance.[65][66]

The mechanisms of Mindful emotion regulation

Through the initial foundations of attention control training, the focus of attention can more consciously be directed towards emotions that arise. Mindfulness combines this mechanism with a particular quality of attitudinal element,[67] of acceptance and non-judgemental awareness. This can range from acknowledging ‘tightness in the chest’ or ‘increases in heart rate’ as well as thought content and emotions that arise. Subsequently, during mindfulness meditation, difficult emotions that may arise become paired with a compassionate and accepting attitude,[60][68] which may gradually extinguish the fear of experiencing the emotions and any related thoughts. Mindfulness practise may lead to the development of metacognitive insight [67][69] or decentering.[68][70] These concepts relate the experiencing thoughts as they are, which is changeable and transient, and that they are not characteristic of absolute reality.[60] This may lead to increased cognitive flexibility [71] reflecting in more adaptively and consciously choosing mental content to identify with, rather than habitually responding. Alternatively a balanced and non-elaborative awareness of experience is cultivated,[60][68] that is not as easily disrupted by the magnitude of emotions experienced or provocative external events.

Evidence of mindfulness and emotion regulation outcomes

Emotional reactivity can be measured and reflected in brain regions related to the production of emotions.[72] It can also be reflected in tests of attentional performance, indexed in poorer performance in attention related tasks. The regulation of emotional reactivity as initiated by attentional control capacities can be taxing to performance, as attentional resources are limited [48]

Controversies in mindful emotion regulation

It is debated as to whether top-down executive control regions such as the Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC),[81] are required [80] or not [73] to inhibit reactivity of the amygdala activation related to the production of evoked emotional responses. Arguably an initial increase in activation of executive control regions developed during mindfulness training may lessen with increasing mindfulness expertise [82]

Changes in the brain

A 6-week mindfulness based intervention was found to correlate with a significant gray matter increase within the precuneus.[83]

Future directions

A large part of mindfulness research is dependent on technology. As new technology continues to be developed, new imaging techniques will become useful in this field. It would be interesting to use real-time fMRI to help give immediate feedback and guide participants through the programs. It could also be used to more easily train and evaluate mental states during meditation itself.[84] The new technology in the upcoming years offers many exciting potentials for the continued research.

An ancient model of the mind known as the Five-Aggregate Model has been proposed as a theoretical resource that could guide mindfulness interventions.[85] This model comprehensively describes moment-to-moment changes that happen in subjective conscious experience.

Research on other types of meditation

Insight (Vipassana) meditation

Insight meditation has been found to be associated with increased cortical thickness.[86]

Sahaja yoga and mental silence

Sahaja yoga meditation has been shown to correlate with particular brain and brain wave activity.[87][88] Some studies have led to suggestions that Sahaja meditation involves 'switching off' irrelevant brain networks for the maintenance of focused internalized attention and inhibition of inappropriate information.[89]

A study comparing practitioners of Sahaja Yoga meditation with a group of non meditators doing a simple relaxation exercise, measured a drop in skin temperature in the meditators compared to a rise in skin temperature in the non meditators as they relaxed. The researchers noted that all other meditation studies that have observed skin temperature have recorded increases and none have recorded a decrease in skin temperature. This suggests that Sahaja Yoga meditation, being a mental silence approach, may differ both experientially and physiologically from simple relaxation.[90] Sahaja meditators scored above peer group for emotional wellbeing measures on SF-36 ratings.[91]

Kundalini yoga

Kundalini yoga meditation research has found that there "appears to produce structural as well as intensity changes in phenomenological experiences of consciousness",[92] and that multiple regions of the brain are active.

Theoria

Fifteen Carmelite nuns came from the monastery to the laboratory to enter a fMRI machine whilst meditating, allowing scientists there to scan their brains using fMRI while they were in a state known as Unio Mystica (and also Theoria).[93] The documentary film Mystical Brain by Isabelle Raynauld examined this study.[94]

Integrative body-mind training

A study involving the participation of a group of college students, who were asked to use a meditation technique called integrative body-mind training (IBMT involves body relaxation, mental imagery, and mindfulness training), concluded that "meditating may improve the integrity and efficiency of certain connections in the brain" through an increase in their number and robustness.[95] Brain scans showed strong white matter changes in the anterior cingulate cortex.[96]

Transcendental

The first Transcendental Meditation (TM) research studies were conducted at UCLA and Harvard University and published in Science and the American Journal of Physiology in 1970 and 1971.[97] However, much research has been of poor quality,[1][98][99] including a high risk for bias due to the connection of researchers to the TM organization and the selection of subjects with a favorable opinion of TM.[100][101][102] Independent systematic reviews have not found health benefits for TM exceeding those of relaxation and health education.[1][98][101] A 2013 statement from the American Heart Association described the evidence supporting TM as a treatment for hypertension as Level IIB, meaning that TM "may be considered in clinical practice" but that its effectiveness is "unknown/unclear/uncertain or not well-established".

Research on unspecified or multiple types of meditation

Brain activity

The medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices have been found to be relatively deactivated during meditation (experienced meditators using concentration, lovingkindness and choiceless awareness meditation). In addition experienced meditators were found to have stronger coupling between the posterior cingulate, dorsal anterior cingulate, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices both when meditating and when not meditating.[103]

It was also observed that an eight-week MBSR course induced changes in gray matter concentrations.[104] Exploratory whole brain analyses identified significant increases in gray matter concentration in the PCC, TPJ, and the cerebellum. These results suggest that participation in MBSR is associated with changes in gray matter concentration in brain regions involved in learning and memory processes, emotion regulation, self-referential processing, and perspective taking.

Perception

Studies have shown that meditation has both short-term and long-term effects on various perceptual faculties. In 1984 a study showed that meditators have a significantly lower detection threshold for light stimuli of short duration.[105] In 2000 a study of the perception of visual illusions by zen masters, novice meditators, and non-meditators showed statistically significant effects found for the Poggendorff Illusion but not for the Müller-Lyer Illusion. The zen masters experienced a statistically significant reduction in initial illusion (measured as error in millimeters) and a lower decrement in illusion for subsequent trials.[106] Tloczynski has described the theory of mechanism behind the changes in perception that accompany mindfulness meditation thus: "A person who meditates consequently perceives objects more as directly experienced stimuli and less as concepts… With the removal or minimization of cognitive stimuli and generally increasing awareness, meditation can therefore influence both the quality (accuracy) and quantity (detection) of perception."[106] Brown also points to this as a possible explanation of the phenomenon: "[the higher rate of detection of single light flashes] involves quieting some of the higher mental processes which normally obstruct the perception of subtle events." In other words, the practice may temporarily or permanently alter some of the top-down processing involved in filtering subtle events usually deemed noise by the perceptual filters.

Relaxation response

Herbert Benson, founder of the Mind-Body Medical Institute, which is affiliated with Harvard University and several Boston hospitals, reports that meditation induces a host of biochemical and physical changes in the body collectively referred to as the "relaxation response".[107] The relaxation response includes changes in metabolism, heart rate, respiration, blood pressure and brain chemistry. Benson and his team have also done clinical studies at Buddhist monasteries in the Himalayan Mountains.[108] Benson wrote The Relaxation Response to document the benefits of meditation, which in 1975 were not yet widely known.[109]

Calming effects

According to a March 2006 article in Psychological Bulletin, EEG activity begins to slow as a result of the practice of meditation.[110] The human nervous system is composed of a parasympathetic system, which works to regulate heart rate, breathing and other involuntary motor functions, and a sympathetic system, which arouses the body, preparing it for vigorous activity. The National Institutes of Health (NIH) has written, "It is thought that some types of meditation might work by reducing activity in the sympathetic nervous system and increasing activity in the parasympathetic nervous system," or equivalently, that meditation produces a reduction in arousal and increase in relaxation.

Work stress

A study of GPs attending a meditation workshop found subsequent falls in their Kessler Psychological Distress Scale - 10 (K10) readings.[111]

Flow

Mindfulness meditation, mindfulness of the breath, and related techniques, are intended to train attention for the sake of provoking insight. A wider, more flexible attention span makes it easier to be aware of a situation, easier to be objective in emotionally or morally difficult situations, and easier to achieve a state of responsive, creative awareness or "flow".[112]

Slowing Ageing Process

Some researches were conducted to understand the malleable determinants of cellular aging, which is critical to understanding human longevity. The researchers concluded saying "We have reviewed data linking stress arousal and oxidative stress to telomere shortness. Meditative practices appear to improve the endocrine balance toward positive arousal (high DHEA, lower cortisol) and decrease oxidative stress. Thus, meditation practices may promote mitotic cell longevity both through decreasing stress hormones and oxidative stress and increasing hormones that may protect the telomere."[113][114][115]

Potential adverse effects and limits of meditation

The following is an official statement from the US government-run National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health:

"Meditation is considered to be safe for healthy people. There have been rare reports that meditation could cause or worsen symptoms in people who have certain psychiatric problems, but this question has not been fully researched. People with physical limitations may not be able to participate in certain meditative practices involving physical movement. Individuals with existing mental or physical health conditions should speak with their health care providers prior to starting a meditative practice and make their meditation instructor aware of their condition."[116]

Adverse effects have been reported,[117][118] and may, in some cases, be the result of "improper use of meditation".[119] The NIH advises prospective meditators to "ask about the training and experience of the meditation instructor… [they] are considering."[116]

As with any practice, meditation may also be used to avoid facing ongoing problems or emerging crises in the meditator's life. In such situations, it may instead be helpful to apply mindful attitudes acquired in meditation while actively engaging with current problems.[120][121] According to the NIH, meditation should not be used as a replacement for conventional health care or as a reason to postpone seeing a doctor.[116]

Weaknesses in historic meditation research

A comparison of the effect of various meditation techniques on systolic blood pressure.[1]

In June, 2007 the United States National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH) published an independent, peer-reviewed, meta-analysis of the state of meditation research, conducted by researchers at the University of Alberta Evidence-based Practice Center. The report reviewed 813 studies involving five broad categories of meditation: mantra meditation, mindfulness meditation, yoga, T'ai chi, and Qigong, and included all studies on adults through September 2005, with a particular focus on research pertaining to hypertension, cardiovascular disease, and substance abuse.

The report concluded, "Scientific research on meditation practices does not appear to have a common theoretical perspective and is characterized by poor methodological quality. Firm conclusions on the effects of meditation practices in healthcare cannot be drawn based on the available evidence. Future research on meditation practices must be more rigorous in the design and execution of studies and in the analysis and reporting of results." (p. 6) It noted that there is no theoretical explanation of health effects from meditation common to all meditation techniques.[1]

A version of this report subsequently published in the Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine stated that "Most clinical trials on meditation practices are generally characterized by poor methodological quality with significant threats to validity in every major quality domain assessed". This was the conclusion despite a statistically significant increase in quality of all reviewed meditation research, in general, over time between 1956 and 2005. Of the 400 clinical studies, 10% were found to be good quality. A call was made for rigorous study of meditation.[3] These authors also noted that this finding is not unique to the area of meditation research and that the quality of reporting is a frequent problem in other areas of complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) research and related therapy research domains.

Of more than 3,000 scientific studies that were found in a comprehensive search of 17 relevant databases, only about 4% had randomised controlled trials (RCTs), which are designed to exclude the placebo effect.[1]

A 2013 statement from the American Heart Association evaluated the evidence for the effectiveness of TM as a treatment for hypertension as "unknown/unclear/uncertain or not well-established", and stated: "Because of many negative studies or mixed results and a paucity of available trials... other meditation techniques are not recommended in clinical practice to lower BP at this time."[122]

See also

Notes

    References

    1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Ospina, Maria B.; Bond, Kenneth; Karkhaneh, Mohammad; Tjosvold, Lisa; Vandermeer, Ben; Liang, Yuanyuan; Bialy, Liza; Hooton, Nicola; Buscemi, Nina; Dryden, Donna M.; Klassen, Terry P. (June 2007). "Meditation practices for health: state of the research" (PDF). Evidence Report/technology Assessment (155): 1–263. PMID 17764203.
    2. Lutz, Antoine; Dunne, John D.; Davidson, Richard J. (2007). "Meditation and the Neuroscience of Consciousness: An Introduction". In Zelazo, Philip David; Moscovitch, Morris; Thompson, Evan. The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 499–552. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511816789.020. ISBN 978-0-511-81678-9.
    3. 1 2 Ospina MB, Bond K, Karkhaneh M, et al. (December 2008). "Clinical trials of meditation practices in health care: characteristics and quality". J Altern Complement Med 14 (10): 1199–213. doi:10.1089/acm.2008.0307. PMID 19123875.
    4. 1 2 3 Tang YY, Posner MI (Jan 2013). "Special issue on mindfulness neuroscience". Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 1–3. doi:10.1093/scan/nss104.
    5. Sequeira S (Jan 2014). "Foreword to advances in meditation research: Neuroscience and clinical applications". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1307: v–vi. doi:10.1111/nyas.12305. PMID 24571183.
    6. 1 2 Posner MI, Tang YY, Lynch G (2014). "Mechanisms of white matter change induced by meditation training". Frontiers in Psychology 5 (1220): 297–302. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01220.
    7. 1 2 Holzel BK, Lazar SW, et al. (Nov 2011). "How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science 6 (6): 537–559. doi:10.1177/1745691611419671. PMID 26168376.
    8. 1 2 Gotink RA, Chu P, Busschbach JJ, et al. (Apr 2015). "Standardised Mindfulness-Based Interventions in Healthcare: An Overview of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses of RCTs". PLoS ONE 10 (4): e0124344. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124344. PMC 4400080. PMID 25881019.
    9. Goyal, M; Singh, S; Sibinga, E. M.; Gould, N. F.; Rowland-Seymour, A; Sharma, R; Berger, Z; Sleicher, D; Maron, D. D.; Shihab, H. M.; Ranasinghe, P. D.; Linn, S; Saha, S; Bass, E. B.; Haythornthwaite, J. A. (2014). "Meditation Programs for Psychological Stress and Well-being: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis". JAMA Internal Medicine 174 (3): 357–68. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018. PMID 24395196. (Full text PDF, 439 pp, 12MB)
    10. 1 2 Strauss C, Cavanagh K, Oliver A, Pettman D (Apr 2014). "Mindfulness-Based Interventions for People Diagnosed with a Current Episode of an Anxiety or Depressive Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Randomised Controlled Trials". PLoS ONE 9 (4): e96110. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096110. PMC 3999148. PMID 24763812.
    11. 1 2 3 Khoury B, Sharma M, Rush SE, Fournier C (Jun 2015). "Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals: A meta-analysis". J Psychosom Res 78 (6): 519–528. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.03.009. PMID 25818837.
    12. Jain FA, Walsh RN, Eisendrath SJ, et al. (2014). "Critical Analysis of the Efficacy of Meditation Therapies for Acute and Subacute Phase Treatment of Depressive Disorders: A systematic Review". Psychosomatics 56 (2): 297–302. doi:10.1016/j.psym.2014.10.007.
    13. Simkin DR, Black NB (July 2014). "Meditation and mindfulness in clinical practice". Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 23 (3): 487–534. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2014.03.002. PMID 24975623.
    14. Zoogman S, Goldberg SB, Hoyt WT (Jan 2014). "Mindfulness Interventions with Youth: A Meta-Analysis". Mindfulness 59 (4): 297–302. doi:10.1093/sw/swu030.
    15. Sharma M, Rush SE (Jul 2014). "Mindfulness-based stress reduction as a stress management intervention for healthy individuals: a systematic review". J Evid Based Complementary Altern Med 19 (4): 271–86. doi:10.1177/2156587214543143. PMID 25053754.
    16. 1 2 Hofmann SG, Sawyer AT, Witt AA, et al. (Apr 2010). "The effect of mindfulness based therapy on anxiety and depression: a meta-analytic review". J Cons Clin Psych 78 (2): 169–183. doi:10.1037/a0018555.
    17. Chiesa A (Apr 2014). "Are mindfulness-based interventions effective for substance use disorders? A systematic review of the evidence". Subst Use Misuse 49 (5): 492–512. doi:10.3109/10826084.2013.770027. PMID 23461667.
    18. 1 2 Garland EL (Jan 2014). "Mindfulness training targets neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface". Front Psychiatry 4 (173). doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00173.
    19. Black DS (Apr 2014). "Mindfulness-based interventions: an antidote to suffering in the context of substance use, misuse, and addiction". Subst Use Misuse 49 (5): 487–91. doi:10.3109/10826084.2014.860749. PMID 24611846.
    20. Huang HP, He M, Wang HY, Zhou M (Jun 2015). "A meta-analysis of the benefits of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on psychological function among breast cancer (BC) survivors". J Psychosom Res 78 (6): 519–528. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.03.009. PMID 25818837.
    21. Godfrey KM, Gallo LC, Afari N (Apr 2015). "Mindfulness-based interventions for binge eating: a systematic review and meta-analysis". J Behav Med 38 (2): 348–62. doi:10.1007/s10865-014-9610-5. PMID 25417199.
    22. Olson KL, Emery CF (Jan 2015). "Mindfulness and weight loss: a systematic review". Psychosom Med 77 (1): 59–67. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000127. PMID 25490697.
    23. Shonin E, Van Gordon W, Griffiths MD (Feb 2014). "Do mindfulness-based therapies have a role in the treatment of psychosis?". Aust N Z J Psychiatry 48 (2): 124–127. doi:10.1177/0004867413512688. PMID 24220133.
    24. Chadwick P (May 2014). "Mindfulness for psychosis". The British Journal of Psychiatry 204 (5): 333–334. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.113.136044.
    25. Khoury B, Lecomte T, Gaudiano BA, et al. (Oct 2013). "Mindfulness interventions for psychosis: a meta-analysis". Schizophrenia Research 150 (1): 176–84. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.055. PMID 23954146.
    26. 1 2 Querstret D, Cropley M. (2013). "Assessing treatments used to reduce rumination and/or worry: A systematic review". Clinical Psychology Review 33 (8): 996–1009. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.004. PMID 24036088.
    27. Gu J, Strauss C, Bond R, Cavanagh K (Apr 2015). "How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies". Clin Psychol Rev 37: 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006. PMID 25689576.
    28. Santarnecchi E, D'Arista S, Egiziano E, et al. (Oct 2014). "Interaction between Neuroanatomical and Psychological Changes after Mindfulness-Based Training". PLoS ONE 9 (10): e108359. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...9j8359S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0108359. PMC 4203679. PMID 25330321.
    29. Crescentini, C; Capurso, V (2015). "Mindfulness meditation and explicit and implicit indicators of personality and self-concept changes". Front Psychol 6: 44. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00044. PMC 4310269. PMID 25688222.
    30. Crescentini, Cristiano; Matiz, Alessio; Fabbro, Franco (2015). "Improving Personality/Character Traits in Individuals with Alcohol Dependence: The Influence of Mindfulness-Oriented Meditation". Journal of Addictive Diseases 34 (1): 75–87. doi:10.1080/10550887.2014.991657. PMID 25585050.
    31. "Mindfulness Research Guide".
    32. Rapgay, Lobsang; Bystrisky, Alexander (2009). "Classical Mindfulness". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1172 (1): 148–62. Bibcode:2009NYASA1172..148R. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04405.x. PMID 19735247.
    33. "Research Spotlight: Mindfulness Meditation Is Associated With Structural Changes in the Brain". NCCIH. January 30, 2011.
    34. Zeidan, F.; Grant, J.A.; Brown, C.A.; McHaffie, J.G.; Coghill, R.C. (June 2012). "Mindfulness meditation-related pain relief: Evidence for unique brain mechanisms in the regulation of pain". Neuroscience Letters 520 (2): 165–173. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.082.
    35. Jensen, Mark P.; Day, Melissa A.; Miró, Jordi (18 February 2014). "Neuromodulatory treatments for chronic pain: efficacy and mechanisms". Nature Reviews Neurology 10 (3): 167–178. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2014.12.
    36. Malinowski, Peter (2013). "Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation". Frontiers in Neuroscience 7. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00008.
    37. Hölzel BK, Carmody J, Evans KC; et al. (March 2010). "Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala". Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5 (1): 11–7. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp034. PMC 2840837. PMID 19776221.
    38. Hölzel, B. K.; Carmody, J; Vangel, M; Congleton, C; Yerramsetti, S. M.; Gard, T; Lazar, S. W. (2011). "Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density". Psychiatry Res 191 (1): 36–43. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006. PMC 3004979. PMID 21071182.
    39. Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness‐based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clinical psychology: Science and practice, 10(2), 144-156.
    40. 1 2 Lutz A., Slagter H. A., Dunne J. D., Davidson R. J. (2008). "Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (4): 163–169. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005. PMC 2693206. PMID 18329323.
    41. 1 2 Moore A., Malinowski P. (2009). "Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility". Consciousness and Cognition 18 (1): 176–186. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008. PMID 19181542.
    42. Kashdan T. B., Rottenberg J. (2010). "Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect ofhealth". Clinical Psychology Review 30 (7): 865–878. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001. PMC 2998793. PMID 21151705.
    43. 1 2 3 Malinowski, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in neuroscience, 7.
    44. Mason M. F., Norton M. I., Van Horn J. D., Wegner D. M., Grafton S. T., Macrae C. N. (2007). "Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought". Science 315 (5810): 393–395. Bibcode:2007Sci...315..393M. doi:10.1126/science.1131295. PMC 1821121. PMID 17234951.
    45. Buckner R.L., Andrews-Hanna J.R., Schacter D.L. (2008). "The brain's default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124: 1–38. Bibcode:2008NYASA1124....1B. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011. PMID 18400922.
    46. Seeley W. W., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F., Keller J., Glover G. H., Kenna H., Greicius M. D. (2007). "Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control". The Journal of Neuroscience 27 (9): 2349–2356. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5587-06.2007. PMC 2680293. PMID 17329432.
    47. Fan J., McCandliss B. D., Fossella J., Flombaum J. I., Posner M. I. (2005). "The activation of attentional networks". NeuroImage 26 (2): 471–479. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004. PMID 15907304.
    48. 1 2 Posner M. I., Rothbart M. K. (2007). "Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science". Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58: 1–23. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516. PMID 17029565.
    49. Schmertz S. K., Anderson P. L., Robins D. L. (2009). "The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 31 (1): 60–66. doi:10.1007/s10862-008-9086-0.
    50. Electroencephalography
    51. Slagter H. A., Lutz A., Greischar L. L., Francis A. D., Nieuwenhuis S., Davis J. M., Davidson R. J. (2007). "Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources". PLOS Biology 5 (6): e138. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138. PMC 1865565. PMID 17488185.
    52. 1 2 3 Jha A. P., Krompinger J., Baime M. J. (2007). "Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 7 (2): 109–119. doi:10.3758/cabn.7.2.109.
    53. 1 2 Fan J., McCandliss B.D., Sommer T., Raz A., Posner M.I. (2002). "Testing the efficiency and independence of attentionalnetworks". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14 (3): 340–347. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886. PMID 11970796.
    54. Van , den Hurk P. A., Giommi F., Gielen S. C., Speckens A. E., Barendregt H. P. (2010). "Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (6): 1168–1180. doi:10.1080/17470210903249365. PMID 20509209.
    55. 1 2 Tang Y.Y., Ma Y., Wang J., Fan Y., Feng S., Lu Q., Posner M.I. (2007). "Short-term meditation training improves attentionand self-regulation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104 (43): 17152–17156. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707678104. PMC 2040428. PMID 17940025.
    56. Chan D., Woollacott M. (2007). "Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the efficiency of executive ororientation networks improved?". Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 13 (6): 651–657. doi:10.1089/acm.2007.7022.
    57. Anderson N.D., Lau M.A., Segal Z.V., Bishop S.R. (2007). "Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control". Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 14 (6): 449–463. doi:10.1002/cpp.544.
    58. Hölzel B. K., Lazar S. W., Gard T., Schuman-Olivier Z., Vago D. R., Ott U. (2011). "How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science 6 (6): 537–559. doi:10.1177/1745691611419671. PMID 26168376.
    59. Moore, A., Gruber, T., Derose, J., & Malinowski, P. (2012). Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 6.
    60. 1 2 3 4 Chambers R., Lo B. C. Y., Allen N. B. (2008). "The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect". Cognitive Therapy and Research 32 (3): 303–322. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0.
    61. 1 2 Hayes A. M., Feldman G. (2004). "Clarifying the construct of mindfulness in the context of emotion regulation and the process of change in therapy". Clinical Psychology: science and practice 11 (3): 255–262. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph080.
    62. Treynor W., Gonzalez R., Nolen-Hoeksema S. (2003). "Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis". Cognitive Therapy and Research 27 (3): 247–259. doi:10.1023/a:1023910315561.
    63. Kumar S., Feldman G., Hayes A. (2008). "Changes in mindfulness and emotion regulation in an exposure-based cognitive therapy for depression". Cognitive Therapy and Research 32 (6): 734–744. doi:10.1007/s10608-008-9190-1.
    64. Ramel W., Goldin P. R., Carmona P. E., McQuaid J. R. (2004). "The effects of mindfulness meditation on cognitive processes and affect in patients with past depression". Cognitive Therapy and Research 28 (4): 433–455.
    65. Baer R. A. (2003). "Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review". Clinical psychology: Science and practice 10 (2): 125–143. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015.
    66. Kabat-Zinn J., Massion A. O., Kristeller J., Peterson L. G., Fletcher K. E., Pbert L.; et al. (1992). "Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders". American Journal of Psychiatry 149 (7): 936–944. doi:10.1176/ajp.149.7.936.
    67. 1 2 Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., ... & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical psychology: Science and practice, 11(3), 230-241.
    68. 1 2 3 Desbordes, G., Gard, T., Hoge, E. A., Hölzel, B. K., Kerr, C., Lazar, S. W., ... & Vago, D. R. (2014). Moving beyond mindfulness: defining equanimity as an outcome measure in meditation and contemplative research. Mindfulness, 1-17.
    69. Teasdale J. D. (1999). "Metacognition, mindfulness and the modification of mood disorders". Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 6 (2): 146–155. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0879(199905)6:2<146::aid-cpp195>3.0.co;2-e.
    70. Masuda A., Hayes S. C., Sackett C. F., Twohig M. P. (2004). "Cognitive defusion and self-relevant negative thoughts: Examining the impact of a ninety-year-old technique". Behaviour Research and Therapy 42 (4): 477–485. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.10.008. PMID 14998740.
    71. Hayes, S. C. (2003). Mindfulness: method and process. Clinical Psychology: Science andPractice, 10(2), 161−165.
    72. Ochsner K.N., Gross J.J. (2005). "The cognitive control of emotion". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (5): 242–249. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. PMID 15866151.
    73. 1 2 Goldin P. R., Gross J. J. (2010). "Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder". Emotion 10 (1): 83–91. doi:10.1037/a0018441. PMC 4203918. PMID 20141305.
    74. Cuthbert B. N., Schupp H. T., Bradley M. M., Birbaumer N., Lang P. J. (2000). "Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report". Biological Psychology 52 (2): 95–111. doi:10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00044-7. PMID 10699350.
    75. Schupp H.T., Cuthbert B.N., Bradley M.M., Cacioppo J.T., Ito T., Lang P.J. (2000). "Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance". Psychophysiology 37 (2): 257–61. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3720257. PMID 10731776.
    76. Schupp H.T., Jungho , Weike A.I., Hamm A.O. (2003). "Attention and emotion: AnERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing". NeuroReport 14 (8): 1107–10. doi:10.1097/00001756-200306110-00002. PMID 12821791.
    77. Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., & Inzlicht, M. (2012). Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nss004.
    78. Ortner C. N., Kilner S. J., Zelazo P. D. (2007). "Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task". Motivation and Emotion 31 (4): 271–283. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9076-7.
    79. Goldin P., Ziv M., Jazaieri H., Gross J.J. (2013). "MBSR vs. aerobic exercise in socialanxiety: fMRI of emotion regulation of negative self-beliefs". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 65–72. doi:10.1093/scan/nss054. PMC 3541489. PMID 22586252.
    80. 1 2 Farb N.A.S., Segal Z.V., Mayberg H., Bean J., McKeon D., Fatima Z., Anderson A.K. (2007). "Attending to the present:Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of selfreference". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2 (4): 313–322. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm030. PMC 2566754. PMID 18985137.
    81. Quirk G. J., Beer J. S. (2006). "Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: Convergence of rat and human studies". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16 (6): 723–727. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.07.004. PMID 17084617.
    82. Chiesa A., Calati R., Serretti A. (2011). "Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings". Clinical Psychology Review 31 (3): 449–464. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003. PMID 21183265.
    83. Kurth, F; Luders, E; Wu, B; Black, D. S. (2014). "Brain Gray Matter Changes Associated with Mindfulness Meditation in Older Adults: An Exploratory Pilot Study using Voxel-based Morphometry". Neuro 1 (1): 23–26. doi:10.17140/NOJ-1-106. PMC 4306280. PMID 25632405.
    84. Tang, Yi-Yuan; Posner, Michael I. (2013). "Tools of the trade: theory and method in mindfulness neuroscience". Oxford Journals: Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 118–120. doi:10.1093/scan/nss112. Retrieved 22 May 2014.
    85. Karunamuni ND (May 2015). "The Five-Aggregate Model of the Mind". SAGE Open 5 (2). doi:10.1177/2158244015583860.
    86. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH; et al. (November 2005). "Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness". NeuroReport 16 (17): 1893–7. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. PMC 1361002. PMID 16272874.
    87. Aftanas, LI; Golocheikine, SA (September 2001). "Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: high-resolution EEG investigation of meditation". Neuroscience Letters 310 (1): 57–60. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02094-8. PMID 11524157.
    88. Aftanas, Ljubomir; Golosheykin, Semen (June 2005). "Impact of regular meditation practice on EEG activity at rest and during evoked negative emotions". The International Journal of Neuroscience 115 (6): 893–909. doi:10.1080/00207450590897969. PMID 16019582.
    89. Aftanas, LI; Golocheikine, SA (September 2002). "Non-linear dynamic complexity of the human EEG during meditation". Neuroscience Letters 330 (2): 143–6. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00745-0. PMID 12231432.
    90. Manocha, Ramesh; Black, Deborah; Spiro, David; Ryan, Jake; Stough, Con (March 2010). "Changing Definitions of Meditation – Is there a Physiological Corollary? Skin temperature changes of a mental silence orientated form of meditation compared to rest" (PDF). Journal of the International Society of Life Sciences 28 (1): 23–31.
    91. http://www.hindawi.com/journals/ecam/2012/350674/
    92. Venkatesh S, Raju TR, Shivani Y, Tompkins G, Meti BL (April 1997). "A study of structure of phenomenology of consciousness in meditative and non-meditative states". Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 41 (2): 149–53. PMID 9142560.
    93. Beauregard, Mario; Paquette, Vincent (September 2006). "Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns". Neuroscience Letters 405 (3): 186–90. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.060. PMID 16872743.
    94. Mystical Brain
    95. "Meditation boosts part of brain where ADD, addictions reside". Ars Technica. Retrieved 2010-08-22.
    96. Tang, Yi-Yuan; Lu, Qilin; Geng, Xiujuan; Stein, Elliot A.; Yang, Yihong; Posner, Michael I. (August 2010). "Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 (35): 15649–52. Bibcode:2010PNAS..10715649T. doi:10.1073/pnas.1011043107. JSTOR 27862304. PMC 2932577. PMID 20713717. Lay summary ScienceDaily (18 August 2010).
    97. Lyn Freeman, Mosby’s Complementary & Alternative Medicine: A Research-Based Approach, Mosby Elsevier, 2009, p. 163
    98. 1 2 Krisanaprakornkit, Thawatchai; Sriraj, Wimonrat; Piyavhatkul, Nawanant; Laopaiboon, Malinee (2006). "Meditation therapy for anxiety disorders". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (1): CD004998. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD004998.pub2. PMID 16437509.
    99. Ernst E (2011). Bonow RO; et al., eds. Chapter 51: Complementary and Alternative Approaches to Management of Patients with Heart Disease. Braunwald's Heart Disease: A Textbook of Cardiovascular Medicine (9th ed.) (Saunders). ISBN 978-1-4377-2708-1. A systematic review of six RCTs of transcendental meditation failed to generate convincing evidence that meditation is an effective treatment for hypertension (References the same 2004 systematic review by Canter and Ernst on TM and hypertension that is separately referenced in this article)
    100. Canter, Peter H; Ernst, Edzard (November 2004). "Insufficient evidence to conclude whether or not Transcendental Meditation decreases blood pressure: results of a systematic review of randomized clinical trials". Journal of Hypertension 22 (11): 2049–54. doi:10.1097/00004872-200411000-00002. PMID 15480084.
    101. 1 2 Krisanaprakornkit, Thawatchai; Ngamjarus, Chetta; Witoonchart, Chartree; Piyavhatkul, Nawanant (2010). "Meditation therapies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (6): CD006507. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD006507.pub2. PMID 20556767.
    102. Canter, Peter H.; Ernst, Edzard (November 2003). "The cumulative effects of Transcendental Meditation on cognitive function — a systematic review of randomised controlled trials". Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift 115 (21–22): 758–66. doi:10.1007/BF03040500. PMID 14743579.
    103. Brewer, Judson A.; Worhunsky, Patrick D.; Gray, Jeremy R.; Tang, Yi-Yuan; Weber, Jochen; Kober, Hedy (December 2011). "Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 (50): 20254–9. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10820254B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112029108. JSTOR 23060108. PMC 3250176. PMID 22114193.
    104. Hölzel, B. K.; Carmody, J; Vangel, M; Congleton, C; Yerramsetti, S. M.; Gard, T; Lazar, S. W. (2011). "Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density". Psychiatry Res 191 (1): 36–43. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006. PMC 3004979. PMID 21071182.
    105. Brown, Daniel; Forte, Michael; Dysart, Michael (June 1984). "Differences in visual sensitivity among mindfulness meditators and non-meditators". Perceptual and Motor Skills 58 (3): 727–33. doi:10.2466/pms.1984.58.3.727. PMID 6382144.
    106. 1 2 Tloczynski, Joseph; Santucci, Aimee; Astor-Stetson, Eileen (December 2000). "Perception of visual illusions by novice and longer-term meditators". Perceptual and Motor Skills 91 (3 Pt 1): 1021–6. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.1021. PMID 11153836.
    107. Benson, Herbert (December 1997). "The relaxation response: therapeutic effect". Science 278 (5344): 1693–7. Bibcode:1997Sci...278.1693B. doi:10.1126/science.278.5344.1693b. PMID 9411784.
    108. Cromie, William J. (18 April 2002). "Meditation changes temperatures: Mind controls body in extreme experiments". Harvard University Gazette. Archived from the original on 24 May 2007.
    109. Benson, Herbert (2001). The Relaxation Response. HarperCollins. pp. 61–3. ISBN 0-380-81595-8.
    110. Cahn, B. Rael; Polich, John (March 2006). "Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies". Psychological Bulletin 132 (2): 180–211. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180. PMID 16536641.
    111. Manoch, Ramesh; Gordon, Amy; Black, Deborah; Malhi, Gin; Seidler, Raymond (June 2009). "Using meditation for less stress and better wellbeing - A seminar for GPs". Australian Family Physician 38 (6): 454–8. PMID 19530378.
    112. Marr, Arthur J. (April 2001). "Commentary: In the Zone: A Biobehavioral Theory of the Flow Experience". Athletic Insight 3 (1).
    113. Epel E, Daubenmier J, Moskowitz JT, Folkman S, Blackburn E (August 2009). "Can meditation slow rate of cellular aging? Cognitive stress, mindfulness, and telomeres". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1172: 34–53. Bibcode:2009NYASA1172...34E. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04414.x. PMC 3057175. PMID 19735238.
    114. 1 2 3 "Meditation: An Introduction". National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. June 2010.
    115. Perez-De-Albeniz, Alberto; Holmes, Jeremy (2000). "Meditation: Concepts, effects and uses in therapy". International Journal of Psychotherapy 5 (1): 49–58. doi:10.1080/13569080050020263.
    116. Rocha, Tomas (25 June 2014). "The Dark Knight of the Soul". The Atlantic.
    117. Turner, Robert P.; Lukoff, David; Barnhouse, Ruth Tiffany; Lu, Francis G. (July 1995). "Religious or spiritual problem. A culturally sensitive diagnostic category in the DSM-IV". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 183 (7): 435–44. doi:10.1097/00005053-199507000-00003. PMID 7623015.
    118. Hayes, 1999, chap. 3
    119. Metzner, 2005
    120. Brook, Robert D.; Appel, Lawrence J.; Rubenfire, Melvyn; Ogedegbe, Gbenga; Bisognano, John D.; Elliott, William J.; Fuchs, Flavio D.; Hughes, Joel W.; Lackland, Daniel T.; Staffileno, Beth A.; Townsend, Raymond R.; Rajagopalan, Sanjay (June 2013). "Beyond medications and diet: alternative approaches to lowering blood pressure: a scientific statement from the american heart association". Hypertension 61 (6): 1360–83. doi:10.1161/HYP.0b013e318293645f. PMID 23608661.

    External links

    This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Wednesday, February 10, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.