Fan effect

Idea Networking Example

The fan effect[1] is a psychological phenomenon under the branch of cognitive psychology where recognition times or error rate for a particular concept increases as more information about the concept is acquired. The word "fan" refers to the number of associations correlated with the concept.

The Origin of the Fan Effect

The fan effect made its first appearance in a series of experiments conducted by John R. Anderson, a cognitive psychologist, in 1974.[2] The three experiments he conducted involved participants learning 26 sentences that paired a person with a location. Additionally, they were asked to determine whether or not a particular sentence that was given to them belonged to the 26 they were asked to study. An example of a sentence Anderson used in his experiment was: "A hippie is in the park." Some sentences seemed similar in the sense that a person was paired with another location. For instance, "A hippie is in the church." Results revealed that participants produced a longer retrieval time when a person was paired with more than one location. Overall, these experiments demonstrated that multiple associations, such as including a large number of nouns in a sentence, interfered with the recognition time by producing a much slower effect.[3]

Spreading Activation

In the brain, memory stores information in a network of nodes that are linked together.[4] When a memory is retrieved, activation spreads on the links until it intersects or enough time has passed. If there is one association, the activation only has to spread to a single link whereas multiple associations would divide the activation to many links. Because there are so many connections, it causes the brain to take a longer time to identify the concepts and retrieve the memory.

Plausible Theories

The fan effect is due to multiple mental models and is included as part of the ACT-R theory.[5] The key factors that the fan effect is dependent on are the strength and degree to which one of the variables can predict the other and the importance of the concept to a person during the retrieval process. Concepts can be better recognized by similar ideas instead of a random order of ideas. When stored in a random order, the concepts are placed in independent places in the brain instead of putting the concepts together as one unit. The fan effect can be reduced if random sentences are exposed frequently and unified into one concept.[6]

References

  1. Anderson, John (October 1974). "Retrieval of propositional information from long-term memory". Cognitive Psychology 6 (4): 451–474. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90021-8.
  2. Radvansky, Gabriel A. (June 1999). "The Fan Effect: A Tale of Two Theories". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128 (2): 198–206. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.2.198.
  3. Buntinga, M.F.,Conwayb, A., and Heitzc, R.P. (April 2004). "Individual differences in the fan effect and working memory capacity". Journal of Memory and Language 51 (4): 604–621. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.007.
  4. Radvansky and Zacks, Gabriel and Rose (September 1991). "Mental Models and the Fan Effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17 (5): 940–953. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.940.
  5. Anderson and Reder, John and Lynne (June 1999). "The fan effect: New results and New Theories". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128 (2): 189–197. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.2.186.
  6. Moeser, Shannon Dawn (March 1979). "The Role of Experimental Design in Investigations of the Fan Effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 5 (2): 125–134. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.5.2.125.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Monday, January 25, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.