Delayed Auditory Feedback

Delayed Auditory Feedback (DAF), also called delayed sidetone, is a type of altered auditory feedback that consists of extending the time between speech and auditory perception.[1] It can consist of a device that enables a user to speak into a microphone and then hear his or her voice in headphones a fraction of a second later. Some DAF devices are hardware; DAF computer software is also available. Most delays that produce a noticeable effect are between 50-200ms. DAF usage (with a 175 millisecond delay) has been shown to induce mental stress.[2]

It is type of altered auditory feedback along with frequency-altered feedback and white noise masking, used to treat stuttering, but also has demonstrated interesting discoveries about the auditory feedback system when used with non-stuttering individuals. It is most effective when used in both ears.

Delayed auditory feedback devices are used for example in speech perception experiments, in order to demonstrate the importance of auditory feedback in speech perception as well as in speech production.[3]

Delayed auditory feedback has been used with a directional microphone and speaker to create a device[4] intended to silence an individual speaker using the mental stress induced in people not used to the effect.[5]

There are now also different mobile apps available that use DAF in phone calls.

Effects in Stutterers

Electronic fluency devices use delayed auditory feedback and have been used as a technique to aid with stuttering. Early investigators suggested and have been continually been proven correct in assuming that those who stutter had an abnormal speech–auditory feedback loop that was corrected or bypassed while speaking under DAF. In stutterers with atypical auditory anatomy, DAF improves fluency, but not in those with typical anatomy. DAF is also used with clutterers. Its effects are slowing of speech with can result in increased fluency for clutterers and also syllable awareness.[6]

Effects in Normal Speakers

Studies that are more recent have looked at the effects of DAF in non-stutterers to see what it can prove about the structure of the auditory and verbal pathways in the brain.

Indirect effects of delayed auditory feedback in non-stutterers include reduction in rate of speech, increase in intensity, and increase in fundamental frequency in order to overcome the effects of the feedback. [7] Direct effects include repetition of syllables, mispronunciations, omissions, and omitted word endings. These direct effects are often referred to as “artificial stuttering” [8]

In a normal individual, auditory feedback speech sounds are directed to the inner ear with a .001 second delay. [9] In delayed auditory feedback, the delay is artificially disrupted.

Studies have found that In children age 4-6 there is less disturbance of speech than children age 7-9 under a delay of 200ms. [10] ). Younger children are maximally disrupted around 500ms while older children around 400ms. A 200ms delay produces maximum disruption for adults. As the data collected from these studies indicates, the delay required for maximum disruption decreases with age. [11] However, it increases again for older adults, to 400ms. [12]

Sex differences in DAF show no difference or indicate that men are generally more affected than women[1] indicating that the feedback subsystems in the vocal monitor process could be different between the sexes. [13]

In general, more rapid, fluent speakers are less affected by DAF than slower, less fluent speakers. Also, more rapid fluent speakers are maximally disrupted by a shorter delay time, while slower speakers are maximally disrupted under longer delay times.

Studies using computational modeling and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have shown that the temporo-parietal regions function as a conscious self-monitoring system to support an automatic speech production system[14] and that projections from auditory error cells in the posterior superior temporal cortex that go to motor correction cells in right frontal cortex mediate auditory feedback control of speech. [15]

Effects in Non-humans


Continuous delayed auditory feedback in Zebra finch songbirds caused them to change their song syllable timing, indicating that DAF can change the motor program of syllable timing generation during short periods of time in zebra finches, similar to the effects observed in humans.[16]


References

  1. 1 2 Ball, MJ; Code, C (1997). Instrumental Clinical Phonetics. London: Whurr Publishers. ISBN 1897635184. Retrieved 7 December 2015.
  2. Badian, M.; et al. (1979). "Standardized mental stress in healthy volunteers induced by delayed auditory feedback (DAF)". European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 16 (3): 171–6. doi:10.1007/BF00562057. PMID 499316.
  3. Perkell, J.; et al. (1997). "Speech Motor Control: Acoustic Goals, Saturation Effects, Auditory Feedback and Internal Models". Speech Communication 22 (2–3): 227–250. doi:10.1016/S0167-6393(97)00026-5.
  4. Dydymus, J. T. (3 March 2012). "Japanese develop 'SpeechJammer' gun". Digital Journal. Retrieved 3 March 2012.
  5. Kurihara, K; Tsukada, K. (2012). "SpeechJammer: A System Utilizing Artificial Speech Disturbance with Delayed Auditory Feedback". arXiv:1202.6106 [cs.HC].
  6. Peter Ramig; Darrell Dodge. The Child and Adolescent Stuttering Treatment & Activity Resource Guide. Cengage Learning. p. 60. ISBN 9781435481176.
  7. Fairbanks, G. (1955). "Selective Vocal Effects of Delayed Auditory Feedback". J. Speech Hearing Dis. (20): 333–346.
  8. Lee, BS. "Some effects of side-tone delay". J Acoust Soc Am (22).
  9. Yates, AJ (1963). "Delayed Auditory Feedback". Psychol Bull (60): 213–232. Retrieved 7 December 2015.
  10. Chase, RA; Sutton, S; First, D; Zubin, J (1961). "A developmental study of changes in behavior under delayed auditory feedback". J Genet Psychol (99): 101–12. PMID 13692555. Retrieved 7 December 2015.
  11. MacKay, D.G. (1968). "Metamorphosis of a critical interval: Age-linked changes in the delay in auditory linked changes in the delay in auditory feedback that produces maximal disruption of speech". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America (43): 811–821.
  12. Siegel, GM; Fehst, CA; Garber, SR; Pick, HL (1980). "Delayed Auditory Feedback with Children". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 23: 802–813. doi:10.1044/jshr.2304.802.
  13. Stuart, A; Kalinowski, J (2015). "EFFECT OF DELAYED AUDITORY FEEDBACK, SPEECH RATE, AND SEX ON SPEECH PRODUCTION". Perceptual and Motor Skills 120: 747–765. doi:10.2466/23.25.PMS.120v17x2.
  14. Tourville, JA; Reilly, KJ; Guenther, FH (2008). "Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech". NeuroImage 39 (3): 1429–1443. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.054. Retrieved 7 December 2015.
  15. Hashimoto, Y; Kuniyoshi, SL (2003). "Brain activations during conscious self-monitoring of speech production with delayed auditory feedback: An fMRI study". Human Brain Mapping 20 (1): 22–28. doi:10.1002/hbm.10119.
  16. Margoliash, D; Fukushima, M (2015). "The effects of delayed auditory feedback revealed by bone conduction microphone in adult zebra finches". Scientific Reports 5. doi:10.1038/srep08800.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Saturday, January 23, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.