Crypsis
In ecology, crypsis is the ability of an animal to avoid observation or detection by other animals. It may be either a predation strategy or an antipredator adaptation, and methods include camouflage, nocturnality, subterranean lifestyle, and mimicry. Crypsis can involve visual, olfactory (with pheromones) or auditory camouflage. When it is visual, the term cryptic coloration is sometimes used, though many different methods of camouflage are employed by animals.
Overview
There is a strong evolutionary pressure for animals to blend into their environment or conceal their shape, for prey animals to avoid predators and for predators to be able to avoid detection by prey. Exceptions include large herbivores without natural enemies, brilliantly-colored birds that rely on flight to escape predators, and venomous animals with warning coloration. Cryptic animals include the tawny frogmouth (feather patterning resembles bark), the tuatara (hides in burrows all day; nocturnal), some jellyfish (transparent), the leafy sea dragon, and the flounder (covers itself in sediment).
Varieties of crypsis
Methods of crypsis include (visual) camouflage, nocturnality, and subterranean lifestyle. Camouflage involves a variety of methods, from disruptive coloration to transparency and some forms of mimicry.[1][2][3]
As a strategy, crypsis is used by predators against prey, and by prey against predators.[1]
Crypsis also applies to eggs[4] and pheromone production.[5] Crypsis can in principle involve visual, olfactory or auditory camouflage.[6]
Visual
Many animals have evolved so that they visually resemble their surroundings, using some sort of natural camouflage that may match the color and texture of the surroundings (cryptic coloration) and/or break up the visual outline of the animal itself (disruptive coloration). Such animals may resemble rocks, sand, twigs, leaves, and even bird droppings.[7]
Some animals change colour in changing environments, either seasonally, as in ermine and snowshoe hare, or far more rapidly with chromatophores in their integuments, as in chameleon and cephalopods such as squid.
Countershading, the use of different colors on upper and lower surfaces in graduating tones from a light belly to a darker back, is common in the sea and on land. This is sometimes called Thayer's law, after the American artist Abbott H. Thayer, who published a paper on the form in 1896, explaining that countershading paints out shadows to make solid objects appear flat, reversing the way artists use paint to make flat paintings contain solid objects. Where the background is brighter than can be achieved even with white pigment, counter-illumination in marine animals such as squid can use light to match the background.
Some animals actively camouflage themselves with local materials. The decorator crabs attach plants, animals, small stones or shell fragments to their carapaces, providing camouflage that matches the local environment. Some species preferentially select stinging animals such as sea anemones or noxious plants, benefiting from aposematism as well as, or instead of, crypsis.[8]
Olfactory
Some animals, in both terrestrial and aquatic environments, appear to camouflage their odour, which might otherwise attract predators.[9] Numerous arthropods, both insects and spiders, mimic ants, whether to avoid predation, to hunt ants, or (for example in the Large Blue Butterfly caterpillar) to trick the ants into feeding them.[10] Pirate perch (Aphredoderus sayanus) may exhibit chemical crypsis, making them undetectable to frogs and insects colonizing ponds.[11]
Auditory
Some insects, notably some Noctuid moths (such as the Large Yellow Underwing) and some tiger moths (such as the Garden Tiger), were originally theorized to defend themselves against predation by echolocating bats, both by passively absorbing sound with soft, fur-like body coverings, and by actively creating sounds to mimic echoes from other locations or objects (a "phantom echo" which might therefore represent "auditory crypsis"), with alternative theories about interfering with the bats' echolocation ("jamming").[12][13] Subsequent research has provided evidence for only two functions of moth sounds, neither of which involve "auditory crypsis"; tiger moth species appear to cluster into two distinct groups: one type produces sounds as acoustic aposematism (warning the bats that the moths are unpalatable, e.g.[14]) or are acoustic mimics of unpalatable moths,[15] and another type that uses sonar jamming. In the latter type of moth, detailed analyses failed to support a “phantom echo” mechanism underlying sonar jamming, and instead pointed towards echo interference.[16]
Effects
There is often a self-perpetuating co-evolution, or evolutionary arms race, between the perceptive abilities of animals for whom it is beneficial to be able to detect the cryptic animal, versus the cryptic characteristics of the hiding species. Different aspects of crypsis and sensory abilities may be more or less pronounced in given predator-prey species pairs.
Zoologists need special methods to study cryptic animals including biotelemetry techniques such as radio tracking, mark and recapture, and enclosures or exclosures. Cryptic animals tend to be overlooked in studies of biodiversity and ecological risk assessment.
Examples
-
Australian grasshopper with the shape and coloration of a leaf
-
The coloration of the leaf-nosed viper (Eristicophis macmahonii) blends with sand.
-
This frog facing right, to the upper left of the stick in the rightmost third of the photo, is nearly invisible among dead leaves.[1]
-
Stenodactylus sthenodactylus matches the background of the Judean desert.
-
A common snakeneck turtle covered in camouflaging algae; when resting this animal looks like an alga-covered rock.
-
Arctiid moths like the Garden Tiger, Arctia caja, have furry bodies to absorb sound, and make clicks to jam bat echolocation.
- ^ Amazonia Org. "Amazonia". Amazonia.org. Retrieved 21 August 2012.
References
- 1 2 Zuanon, J.; I. Sazima (2006). "The almost invisible league: crypsis and association between minute fishes and shrimps as a possible defence against visually hunting predators". Neotropical Ichthyology 4 (2): 219–214. doi:10.1590/S1679-62252006000100012.
- ↑ Allaby, Michael (2014). Crypsis. A Dictionary of Zoology (4th ed.) (Oxford University Press).
- ↑ Allaby, Michael (2015). Crypsis. A Dictionary of Ecology (5th ed.) (Oxford University Press).
- ↑ Nguyen, L. P.; et al. (2007). "Using digital photographs to evaluate the effectiveness of plover egg crypsis". Journal of Wildlife Management 71 (6): 2084–2089. doi:10.2193/2006-471.
- ↑ Raffa, K. R.; et al. (2007). "Can chemical communication be cryptic? Adaptations by herbivores to natural enemies exploiting prey semiochemistry". Oecologia 153 (4): 1009–1019. doi:10.1007/s00442-007-0786-z. PMID 17618465.
- ↑ "Definition of Crypsis". Amateur Entomologists' Society. Retrieved 19 August 2012.
- ↑ "All Lives Transform:Adaptation- Mimicry". Morning-earth.org. 2007-02-14. Retrieved 5 January 2012.
- ↑ Hultgren, Kristin; Stachowicz, Jay in Stevens, M and Merilaita, S (2011). "Animal Camouflage" (PDF). Camouflage in decorator crabs: Camouflage in decorator crabs. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved December 13, 2012.
- ↑ Michael R. Conover. Predator-Prey Dynamics: the role of olfaction. CRC Press. 2007. ISBN 978-0-8493-9270-2
- ↑ Donisthorpe, Horace (January 1922). Mimicry of Ants by Other Arthropods. Transactions of the Royal Entomological Society of London. 69, Issue 3-4. pp. 307–311.
- ↑ Resetarits, Jr., William J.; Binckley, Christopher A. (2013). "Is the pirate really a ghost? Evidence for generalized chemical camouflage in an aquatic predator, pirate perch Aphredoderus sayanus". The American Naturalist 181 (5): 690–699. doi:10.1086/670016. Archived from the original (PDF) on May 31, 2013.
- ↑ Miller, Lee A.; Surlykke, Annemarie (July 2001). "How Some Insects Detect and Avoid Being Eaten by Bats: Tactics and Countertactics of Prey and Predator" (PDF). BioScience 51 (7): 570–581. doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2001)051[0570:HSIDAA]2.0.CO;2.
- ↑ Griffin, Donald R. (July 2001). "Full Access Return to the Magic Well: Echolocation Behavior of Bats and Responses of Insect Prey". BioScience 51 (7): 555–556. doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2001)051[0555:RTTMWE]2.0.CO;2.
- ↑ N.I. Hristov, W.E. Conner. 2005. Sound strategy: acoustic aposematism in the bat–tiger moth arms race. Naturwissenschaften 92: 164-169.
- ↑ J.R. Barber, W.E. Conner. 2007. Acoustic mimicry in a predator-prey interaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104:9331-9334.
- ↑ A.J. Corcoran, W.E. Conner, J.R. Barber. 2010. Anti-bat tiger moth sounds: Form and function. Current Zoology 56 (3): 358–369.
External links
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Crypsis. |
- Dive Gallery: decorator crabs.
- Caterpillar that resembles bird droppings on leaves.
|
|