Cognitive dissonance

In psychology, cognitive dissonance is the mental stress or discomfort experienced by an individual who holds two or more contradictory beliefs, ideas, or values at the same time, performs an action that is contradictory to one or more beliefs, ideas or values, or is confronted by new information that conflicts with existing beliefs, ideas, or values.[1][2]

Leon Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance focuses on how humans strive for internal consistency. An individual who experiences inconsistency (dissonance) tends to become psychologically uncomfortable, and is motivated to try to reduce this dissonance—as well as actively avoid situations and information likely to increase it.[1]

Relationship between cognitions

Individuals can adjust their attitudes or actions in various ways. Adjustments result in one of three relationships between two cognitions or between a cognition and a behavior.[1]

Consonant relationship
Two cognitions/actions that are consistent with one another (e.g., not wanting to get intoxicated while out, then ordering water instead of alcohol)
Irrelevant relationship
Two cognitions/actions that are unrelated to one another (e.g., not wanting to get intoxicated while out, then tying your shoes)
Dissonant relationship
Two cognitions/actions that are inconsistent with one another (e.g., not wanting to get intoxicated while out, then consuming a large quantity of alcohol)

Magnitude of dissonance

The amount of dissonance produced by two conflicting cognitions or actions (as well as the subsequent psychological distress) depends on two factors:

  1. The importance of cognitions: The more that the elements are personally valued, the greater the magnitude of the dissonant relationship.
  2. Ratio of cognitions: The proportion of dissonant to consonant elements

The pressure to reduce cognitive dissonance is a function of the magnitude of this dissonance.[1]

Reducing

Cognitive dissonance theory is founded on the assumption that individuals seek consistency between their expectations and their reality. Because of this, people engage in a process called dissonance reduction to bring their cognitions and actions in line with one another. This creation of uniformity allows for a lessening of psychological tension and distress. According to Festinger, dissonance reduction can be achieved in four ways.[1] In an example case where a person has adopted the attitude that they will no longer eat high fat food, but eats a high-fat doughnut, the four methods of reduction are:

  1. Change behavior or cognition ("I will not eat any more of this doughnut")
  2. Justify behavior or cognition by changing the conflicting cognition ("I'm allowed to cheat every once in a while")
  3. Justify behavior or cognition by adding new cognitions ("I'll spend 30 extra minutes at the gym to work this off")
  4. Ignore or deny any information that conflicts with existing beliefs ("This doughnut is not high in fat")

Theory and research

Most of the research on cognitive dissonance takes the form of one of four major paradigms. Important research generated by the theory has been concerned with the consequences of exposure to information inconsistent with a prior belief, what happens after individuals act in ways that are inconsistent with their prior attitudes, what happens after individuals make decisions, and the effects of effort expenditure. A key tenet of cognitive dissonance theory is that those who have heavily invested in a position may, when confronted with disconfirming evidence, go to greater lengths to justify their position.

Belief disconfirmation paradigm

Dissonance is felt when people are confronted with information that is inconsistent with their beliefs. If the dissonance is not reduced by changing one's belief, the dissonance can result in restoring consonance through misperception, rejection or refutation of the information, seeking support from others who share the beliefs, and attempting to persuade others.[3]

An early version of cognitive dissonance theory appeared in Leon Festinger's 1956 book When Prophecy Fails. This book gives an account of the deepening of cult members' faith following the failure of a cult's prophecy that a UFO landing was imminent. The believers met at a pre-determined place and time, believing they alone would survive the Earth's destruction. The appointed time came and passed without incident. They faced acute cognitive dissonance: had they been the victim of a hoax? Had they donated their worldly possessions in vain? Most members chose to believe something less dissonant to resolve reality not meeting their expectations: they believed that the aliens had given Earth a second chance, and the group was now empowered to spread the word that Earth-spoiling must stop. The group dramatically increased their proselytism despite (because of) the failed prophecy.[4]

Another example of the belief disconfirmation paradigm is an orthodox Jewish group that in 1994 believed that their highest ranking Jewish Rabbi might be the Messiah. When the Rabbi died of a stroke, instead of accepting that he was not the Messiah, some of them concluded that he was still the Messiah but would soon be resurrected from the dead.[5] Some have suggested the same process might explain the belief two thousand years ago that Jesus was resurrected from the dead.[6]

Induced-compliance paradigm

In Festinger and Carlsmith's classic 1959 experiment, students were asked to spend an hour on boring and tedious tasks (e.g., turning pegs a quarter turn, over and over again). The tasks were designed to generate a strong, negative attitude. Once the subjects had done this, the experimenters asked some of them to do a simple favour. They were asked to talk to another subject (actually an actor) and persuade the impostor that the tasks were interesting and engaging. Some participants were paid $20 (equivalent to $162 in present day terms[7]) for this favour, another group was paid $1 (equivalent to $8 in present day terms[7]), and a control group was not asked to perform the favour.

After someone has performed dissonant behavior, they may find external consonant elements. A snake oil salesman may find a justification for promoting falsehoods (e.g., large personal gain), but may otherwise need to change his views about the falsehoods themselves.

When asked to rate the boring tasks at the conclusion of the study (not in the presence of the other "subject"), those in the $1 group rated them more positively than those in the $20 and control groups. This was explained by Festinger and Carlsmith as evidence for cognitive dissonance. The researchers theorized that people experienced dissonance between the conflicting cognitions, "I told someone that the task was interesting", and "I actually found it boring." When paid only $1, students were forced to internalize the attitude they were induced to express, because they had no other justification. Those in the $20 condition, however, had an obvious external justification for their behaviour, and thus experienced less dissonance.[8]

In subsequent experiments, an alternative method of inducing dissonance has become common. In this research, experimenters use counter-attitudinal essay-writing, in which people are paid varying amounts of money (e.g., $1 or $10) for writing essays expressing opinions contrary to their own. People paid only a small amount of money have less external justification for their inconsistency, and must produce internal justification to reduce the high degree of dissonance they experience.

A variant of the induced-compliance paradigm is the forbidden toy paradigm. An experiment by Aronson and Carlsmith in 1963 examined self-justification in children.[9] In this experiment, children were left in a room with a variety of toys, including a highly desirable toy steam-shovel (or other toy). Upon leaving the room, the experimenter told half the children that there would be a severe punishment if they played with that particular toy and told the other half that there would be a mild punishment. All of the children in the study refrained from playing with the toy.[9] Later, when the children were told that they could freely play with whatever toy they wanted, the ones in the mild punishment condition were less likely to play with the toy, even though the threat had been removed. The children who were only mildly threatened had to justify to themselves why they did not play with the toy. The degree of punishment by itself was not strong enough—so, to resolve their dissonance, the children had to convince themselves that the toy was not worth playing with.[9]

A 2012 study using a version of the forbidden toy paradigm showed that hearing music reduces the development of cognitive dissonance.[10] With no music playing in the background, the control group of four-year-old children were told to avoid playing with a particular toy. After playing alone, the children later devalued the forbidden toy in their ranking, which is similar findings to earlier studies. However, in the variable group, classical music was played in the background while the children played alone. In that group, the children did not later devalue the toy. The researchers concluded that music may inhibit cognitions that result in dissonance reduction.[10] Music is not the only example of an outside force lessening post-decisional dissonance; a 2010 study showed that hand-washing had a similar effect.[11]

Free-choice paradigm

In a different type of experiment conducted by Jack Brehm, 225 female students rated a series of common appliances and were then allowed to choose one of two appliances to take home as a gift. A second round of ratings showed that the participants increased their ratings of the item they chose, and lowered their ratings of the rejected item.[12]

This can be explained in terms of cognitive dissonance. When making a difficult decision, there are always aspects of the rejected choice that one finds appealing and these features are dissonant with choosing something else. In other words, the cognition, "I chose X" is dissonant with the cognition, "There are some things I like about Y." More recent research has found similar results in four-year-old children and capuchin monkeys.[13]

In addition to internal deliberations, the structuring of decisions among other individuals may play a role in how an individual acts. Researchers in a 2010 study examined social preferences and norms as related, in a linear manner, to wage giving among three individuals. The first participant's actions influenced the second's own wage giving. The researchers argue that inequity aversion is the paramount concern of the participants.[14]

Effort justification paradigm

Further information: Effort justification

Dissonance is aroused whenever individuals voluntarily engage in an unpleasant activity to achieve some desired goal. Dissonance can be reduced by exaggerating the desirability of the goal. Aronson & Mills[15] had individuals undergo a severe or mild "initiation" to join a group. In the severe-initiation condition, the individuals engaged in an embarrassing activity. The group they joined turned out to be dull and boring. The individuals in the severe-initiation condition evaluated the group as more interesting than the individuals in the mild-initiation condition (cf. sunk costs).

All of the above paradigms continue to be used in fruitful research.

Washing one's hands has been shown to eliminate post-decisional dissonance, presumably because the dissonance is commonly caused by moral disgust (with oneself), which is related to disgust from unsanitary conditions.[16][17]

Examples

"The Fox and the Grapes" by Aesop. When the fox fails to reach the grapes, he decides he does not want them after all. Rationalization (making excuses) is often involved in reducing anxiety about conflicting cognitions, according to cognitive dissonance theory.

"The Fox and the Grapes"

A classic illustration of cognitive dissonance is expressed in the fable "The Fox and the Grapes" by Aesop (ca. 620–564 BCE). In the story, a fox sees some high-hanging grapes and wishes to eat them. When the fox is unable to think of a way to reach them, he decides that the grapes are probably not worth eating, with the justification the grapes probably are not ripe or that they are sour (hence the common phrase "sour grapes"). The moral that accompanies the story is "Any fool can despise what he cannot get". This example follows a pattern: one desires something, finds it unattainable, and reduces one's dissonance by criticizing it. Jon Elster calls this pattern "adaptive preference formation".[18]

Other related phenomena

Cognitive dissonance has also been demonstrated to occur when people seek to:

Balance theory suggests people have a general tendency to seek consonance between their views, and the views or characteristics of others (e.g., a religious believer may feel dissonance because their partner does not have the same beliefs as he or she does, thus motivating the believer to justify or rationalize this incongruence). People may self handicap so that any failures during an important task are easier to justify (e.g., the student who drinks the night before an important exam in response to his fear of performing poorly).

Applications of research

In addition to explaining certain counter-intuitive human behaviour, the theory of cognitive dissonance has practical applications in several fields.

Education

An educator might introduce topics by challenging students' intuitions. For instance, a student may be more willing to learn the real cause of the seasons after wrongly guessing that it has something to do with changes in the distance of Earth's orbit from the Sun.

Creating and resolving cognitive dissonance can have a powerful impact on students' motivation for learning.[23] For example, researchers have used the effort justification paradigm to increase students' enthusiasm for educational activities by offering no external reward for students' efforts: preschoolers who completed puzzles with the promise of a reward were less interested in the puzzles later, as compared to preschoolers who were offered no reward in the first place.[24] The researchers concluded that students who can attribute their work to an external reward stop working in the absence of that reward, while those who are forced to attribute their work to intrinsic motivation came to find the task genuinely enjoyable.

Psychologists have incorporated cognitive dissonance into models of basic processes of learning, notably constructivist models. Several educational interventions have been designed to foster dissonance in students by increasing their awareness of conflicts between prior beliefs and new information (e.g., by requiring students to defend prior beliefs) and then providing or guiding students to new, correct explanations that resolve the conflicts.[25]

For example, researchers have developed educational software that uses these principles to facilitate student questioning of complex subject matter.[26] Meta-analytic methods suggest that interventions that provoke cognitive dissonance to achieve directed conceptual change have been demonstrated across numerous studies to significantly increase learning in science and reading.[25]

Therapy

The general effectiveness of psychotherapy and psychological intervention has been explained in part through cognitive dissonance theory.[27] Some social psychologists have argued that the act of freely choosing a specific therapy, together with the effort and money the client invests to continue the chosen therapy, positively influences the effectiveness of therapy.[28] This phenomenon was demonstrated in a study with overweight children, in which causing the children to believe that they freely chose the type of therapy they received resulted in greater weight loss.[29]

In another example, individuals with ophidiophobia (fear of snakes) who invested significant effort to engage in activities without therapeutic value for their condition, but were framed as legitimate and relevant therapy, showed significant improvement in phobic symptoms.[30] In these cases, and perhaps in many similar situations, patients came to feel better as a way to justify their efforts and ratify their choices. Beyond these observed short-term effects, effort expenditure in therapy also predicts long-term therapeutic change.[31]

Promoting healthy and pro-social behavior

It has also been demonstrated that cognitive dissonance can be used to promote behaviours such as increased condom use.[32] Other studies suggest that cognitive dissonance can also be used to encourage individuals to engage in prosocial behaviour under various contexts such as campaigning against littering,[33] reducing prejudice to racial minorities,[34] and compliance with anti-speeding campaigns.[35] The theory can also be used to explain reasons for donating to charity.[36][37]

Consumer behavior

Existing literature suggests that three main conditions exist for arousal of dissonance in purchases: the decision involved in the purchase must be important, such as involvement of a lot of money or psychological cost and be personally relevant to the consumer, the consumer has freedom in selecting among the alternatives, and finally, the decision involvement must be irreversible.[38]

A study performed by Lindsay Mallikin shows that when consumers experience an unexpected price encounter, they adopt three methods to reduce dissonance:[39] Consumers may employ a strategy of constant information, they may have a change in attitude, or they may engage in trivialization. Consumers employ the strategy of constant information by engaging in bias and searching for information that supports their prior beliefs. Consumers might search for information about other retailers and substitute products consistent with their belief states. Alternatively, consumers may show change in attitude such as reevaluating price in relation to external reference prices or associating high or low prices with quality. Lastly, trivialization may occur and the importance of the elements of the dissonant relationship is reduced; consumers tend to trivialize importance of money, and thus of shopping around, saving, and receiving a better deal.

Cognitive dissonance is also useful to explain and manage post-purchase concerns. A consumer who feels an alternate purchase would have been better will likely not buy the product again. To counter this, marketers have to convince buyers constantly that the product satisfies their need and thereby helps reduce their cognitive dissonance, ensuring repurchase of the same brand in the future. An example of post-purchase dissonance resolution used in a client relation is a salesperson congratulating his buyer on "having made the right choice".

At times cognitive dissonance is induced, rather than resolved, to market products. The Hallmark Cards tag line "When you care enough to send the very best" is an example of a marketing strategy that creates guilt in the buyer if he or she goes for a less expensive card. Such aggressive marketing ensures that the recipient also is aware that the product has a premium price. This encourages the consumer to buy the expensive cards on special occasions.

Social engineering

Social engineering as applied to security is the exploitation of various social and psychological weaknesses in individuals and business structures, sometimes for penetration testing but more often for nefarious purposes, such as espionage against businesses, agencies, and individuals, typically toward the end of obtaining some illegal gain, either of useful but restricted or private information or for monetary gain through such methods as phishing to obtain banking account access, or for purposes of identity theft, blackmail, and so forth. Exploitation of weaknesses caused by inducing cognitive dissonance in targets is one of the techniques used by perpetrators.

Challenges and alternative theories

A lawyer may experience the negative tension of dissonance if they must defend, and call "innocent", a client that they think is actually guilty. On Aronson's view, however, the lawyer may feel dissonance specifically because falsely calling the defendant "innocent" conflicts with the lawyer's own self-concept of being an honest person.

While cognitive dissonance theory has been utilized in experiments and is generally (although not entirely) accepted by those in the psychology field, there are alternative theories that account for human attitudes and behaviors.

Self-perception theory (Bem)

Daryl Bem was an early critic of cognitive dissonance theory. He proposed self-perception theory as a more parsimonious alternative explanation of the experimental results. According to Bem, people do not think much about their attitudes, let alone whether they are in conflict. Bem's self-perception theory functions under the notion that people develop attitudes by observing their own behavior and concluding what attitudes caused it. This is particularly true when internal cues are weak or ambiguous. Individuals are in the same position as an observer—meaning they must rely on external cues to infer their own inner state. Self-perception theory suggests people adopt attitudes without accessing internal cognition and mood states.[40]

Bem interpreted people in the Festinger and Carlsmith study or the induced-compliance paradigm as inferring their attitudes from their behavior. Thus, when asked "Did you find the task interesting?" they decided that they must have found it interesting because that is what they told someone. Bem suggested that people who were paid $20 had a salient, external incentive for their behavior and were likely to perceive the money as their reason for saying the task was interesting, rather than concluding that they actually found it interesting.[41][42]

In many experimental situations, Bem's theory and Festinger's dissonance theory make identical predictions, but only dissonance theory predicts the presence of unpleasant tension or arousal. Lab experiments have verified the presence of arousal in dissonance situations.[43][44] This provides support for cognitive dissonance theory and makes it unlikely that self-perception by itself can account for all the laboratory findings.

In 1969, Elliot Aronson reformulated the theory by linking it to the self-concept, clarifying that cognitive dissonance arises from conflicts between cognitions when those conflicts threaten a person's normally positive self-image. Thus, Aronson reinterpreted the findings of the original Festinger and Carlsmith study using the induced-compliance paradigm, stating that the dissonance was between the cognition, "I am an honest person" and the cognition, "I lied to someone about finding the task interesting."[45] Other psychologists have argued that maintaining cognitive consistency is a way to protect public self-image, rather than private self-concept.[46] However, a recent result[47] seems to rule out such an explanation by showing revaluation of items following a choice even when people have forgotten their choices.

Balance theory ("P-O-X" Theory) (Heider)

Fritz Heider proposed a motivational theory of attitude change that functions on the idea that humans are driven to establish and maintain psychological balance. This drive is known as the consistency motive—the urge to maintain one's values and beliefs over time. According to balance theory there are three things interacting: (1) you (P), (2) another person (O), and (3) an element (X). These are each positioned at one point of a triangle and share two relations:[40]

  1. Unit relations – things and people that belong together based on similarity, proximity, fate, etc.
  2. Sentiment relations – evaluations of people and things (liking, disliking)

As individuals, we seek a balanced state with harmonious relations between the three positions (3 positive or 2 negative, 1 positive):

P = you
O = John
X = John's dog
"I don't like John"
"John has a dog"
"I don't like the dog either"

We also avoid unbalanced states (3 negatives or 2 positive, 1 negative)

P = you
O = your child
X = picture your child drew
"I love my child"
"She drew me this picture"
"I love this picture"

Cost-benefit analysis (Dupuit)

Jules Dupuit claims behaviors and cognitions can be understood from an economic standpoint such that individuals engage in the systematic processing and comparison of the costs and benefits of a decision. This process helps justify and assess the feasibility of a decision and provides a basis for comparison (determining if the benefits outweigh the costs and to what extent). Although this analysis works well in economic situations, humans are inefficient when it comes to comparing costs and benefits.[48]

Self-discrepancy theory (Higgins)

E. Tory Higgins proposed that individuals have three selves that they compare themselves to:

  1. Actual self – representation of the attributes you believe you actually possess (basic self-concept)
  2. Ideal self – attributes you would ideally like to possess (hopes, aspiration, what motivates you to change/improve)
  3. Ought self – attributes you believe you should possess (duties, obligations, responsibilities)

When these self-guides are contradictory they result in emotional discomfort. Individuals are motivated to reduce self-discrepancy (the gap between two self-guides).[49]

Averse consequences vs. inconsistency (Cooper & Fazio)

During the 1980s, Cooper and Fazio argued that dissonance was caused by aversive consequences, rather than inconsistency. According to this interpretation, the belief that lying is wrong and hurtful, not the inconsistency between cognitions, is what makes people feel bad.[50] Subsequent research, however, found that people experience dissonance even when they feel they have not done anything wrong. For example, Harmon-Jones and colleagues showed that people experience dissonance even when the consequences of their statements are beneficial—as when they convince sexually active students to use condoms, when they, themselves are not using condoms.[51]

Free-choice paradigm criticism (Chen et al.)

Chen and colleagues have criticized the free-choice paradigm and have suggested that the "Rank, choice, rank" method of studying dissonance is invalid.[52] They argue that research design relies on the assumption that if the subject rates options differently in the second survey, then the subject's attitudes towards the options have therefore changed. They show that there are other reasons one might get different rankings in the second survey — perhaps the subjects were largely indifferent between choices. Although some follow-up studies have found supportive evidence for Chen's concerns,[53] other studies that have controlled for Chen's concerns have not, instead suggesting that the mere act of making a choice can indeed change preferences.[13][54][55] Nevertheless, this issue remains under active investigation.[56]

Action and/or motivation based model (Harmon-Jones)

This model states that inconsistencies in cognitions make people distressed since inconsistencies can interfere with actions. A number of cognitive strategies are then implemented. One may "freely" choose to act in behaviors that are not consistent with a current attitude or belief, but later try to alter their belief to match a current behavior. Dissonance occurs because cognitions do not match actions. If one changes one's attitude after dissonance occurs, one then has an obligation to commit to the behavior. When dissonance happens, the person has a negative affective state that makes them reconsider their behavior to solve the inconsistency that is the problem (Beckmann and Kuhl, 1984, Harmon-Jones, 1999, Harmon-Jones, 2000a, Jones and Gerard, 1967, McGregor et al., 1999 and Newby-Clark et al., 2002).) As a person works towards a commitment, then the motivational process is activated in the left frontal cortex.[57][58][59][60][61]

Neuroscience findings

There is evidence suggesting that the more the anterior cingulate cortex signals conflict, the more dissonance a person experiences and the more their attitudes may change.

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), Van Veen and colleagues investigated the neural basis of cognitive dissonance in a modified version of the classic induced compliance paradigm. While in the scanner, participants "argued" that the uncomfortable MRI environment was nevertheless a pleasant experience. The researchers replicated the basic induced compliance findings; participants in an experimental group enjoyed the scanner more than participants in a control group who simply were paid to make their argument.[62]

Importantly, responding counter-attitudinally activated the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insular cortex; furthermore, the degree to which these regions were activated predicted individual participants' degree of attitude change. Van Veen and colleagues argue that these findings support the original dissonance theory by Festinger, and support the "conflict theory" of anterior cingulate functioning.[62]

Using the free choice paradigm, Sharot and colleagues have shown that after making a choice, activity in the striatum changes to reflect the new evaluation of the choice object, increasing if the object was chosen and decreasing if it was rejected.[63] Follow-up studies have largely confirmed these results.[54][64][65]

Subsequent fMRI studies, also using the free choice paradigm, have examined the decision-making process in the brain. A 2010 study showed that during decision-making processes where the participant is trying to reduce dissonance, activity increased in the right-inferior frontal gyrus, medial fronto-parietal region and ventral striatum, whereas activity decreased in the anterior insula.[65] Researchers concluded that rationalization activity may take place quickly (within seconds) without conscious deliberation. In addition, the researchers stated that the brain may engage emotional responses in the decision-making process.[65]

Cognitive dissonance has been associated with left frontal activity in the cortex (Harmon-Jones, 1999 and Harmon-Jones and Harmon-Jones, 2002). In addition, the left frontal cortex has been associated with anger, with anger supporting a motivational purpose behind its anger showing the left frontal activity being active. Together, cognitive dissonance and anger are supported with the motivational directional model. Approach motivation is associated with the left frontal cortex when it can be detected that a person may able to take control of a situation that may have made them angry. Conversely, if a person does not have control of changing the situation, then there is no motivation involved and other emotions may arise. (See Approach motivation model) [58][66][67]

The anterior cingulate cortex activity increases when errors occur and are being monitored as well as having behavioral conflicts with the self-concept as a form of higher-level thinking (Amodio et al., 2004). A study was done to test the prediction that the left frontal cortex would have increased activity. University students had to write a paper depending on if they were assigned to a high-choice or low-choice condition. The low-choice condition required student to write about supporting a 10% increase in tuition at their university. The point of this condition was to see how significant the counterchoice may affect a person ability to cope. The high-choice condition asked students to write in favor of tuition increase as if it was their choice and that it was completely voluntary. EEG was used to analyze students before writing the essay as dissonance is at its highest during this time (Beauvois and Joule, 1996). High-choice condition participants showed a higher level of the left frontal cortex than the low-choice participants. Results have shown that the initial experience of dissonance can be apparent in the anterior cingulate cortex, then the left frontal cortex is activated, which also activates the approach motivational system to reduce anger.[68][69]

There may be evolutionary forces behind cognitive dissonance reduction. Researchers in a 2007 study examined how preschool children and Capuchin monkeys reacted when offered the choice between two similar options. The researchers had the two subject groups choose between two different kinds of stickers and candies. After choosing, the two groups were offered a new choice between the item not chosen and a similarly attractive option as the first. In line with cognitive dissonance theory, the children and the monkeys chose the "novel" option over their originally unchosen option, even though all had similar values. The researchers concluded that there were possible development and evolutionary forces behind cognitive dissonance reduction.[70]

Modeling in neural networks

Neural network models of cognition have provided the necessary framework to integrate the empirical research done on cognitive dissonance and attitudes into one model of explanation of attitude formation and change.[71]

Various neural network models have been developed to predict how cognitive dissonance influence an individual's attitude and behavior. These include:

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. California: Stanford University Press.
  2. Festinger, L. (1962). "Cognitive dissonance". Scientific American 207 (4): 93–107. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1062-93.
  3. Harmon-Jones, Eddie, A Cognitive Dissonance Theory Perspective on Persuasion, in The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice, James Price Dillard, Michael Pfau, eds. 2002. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, p.101.
  4. Festinger, L., Riecken, H.W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  5. Berger, David (2008). The Rebbe, the Messiah, and the Scandal of Orthodox Indifference. Portland: Litman Library of Jewish Civilization.
  6. Komarnitsky, Kris (2014). "Cognitive Dissonance and the Resurrection of Jesus". The Fourth R magazine, Volume 27, Issue 5 (September/October 2014).
  7. 1 2 Consumer Price Index (estimate) 1800–. Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis. Retrieved November 10, 2015.
  8. Festinger, L.; Carlsmith, J.M. (1959). "Cognitive consequences of forced compliance". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 58 (2): 203–210. doi:10.1037/h0041593.
  9. 1 2 3 Aronson, E.; Carlsmith, J.M. (1963). "Effect of the severity of threat on the devaluation of forbidden behavior". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 66 (6): 584–588. doi:10.1037/h0039901.
  10. 1 2 Masataka, Nobuo; Perlovsky, Leonid (2012). "Music can reduce cognitive dissonance". Nature 2. doi:10.1038/srep00694.
  11. Lee, Spike W. S.; Schwarz, Norbert (May 2010). "Washing Away Postdecisional Dissonance". Science 328 (5979): 709. doi:10.1126/science.1186799. PMID 20448177.
  12. Brehm, J. (1956). "Post-decision changes in desirability of alternatives". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 52 (3): 384–389. doi:10.1037/h0041006.
  13. 1 2 Egan, L.C.; Bloom, P.; Santos, L.R. (2010). "Choice-induced preferences in the absence of choice: Evidence from a blind two choice paradigm with young children and capuchin monkeys". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (1): 204–207. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.014.
  14. Gachter, S.; Nosenzo, D.; Sefton, M. (2010). "Peer effects in pro-social behavior: Social norms or social preferences?". CeDEx Discussion Paper Series 2010-23. The University of Nottingham. Retrieved 12 October 2012.
  15. Aronson, E.; Mills, J. (1956). "The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group" (PDF). Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59: 177–181. doi:10.1037/h0047195.
  16. Lee, S.W.S.; Schwartz, N. (2010). "Washing away postdecisional dissonance". Science 328 (5979): 709. doi:10.1126/science.1186799. PMID 20448177.
  17. Zhong, C.B.; Liljenquist, K. (2006). "Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing". Science 313 (5792): 1451–1452. doi:10.1126/science.1130726. PMID 16960010.
  18. Elster, Jon. Sour Grapes: Studies in the Subversion of Rationality. Cambridge 1983, p. 123ff.
  19. Prasad, J. (1950). "A comparative study of rumours and reports in earthquakes". British Journal of Psychology 41 (3-4): 129–144. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1950.tb00271.x.
  20. Knox, R. E., & Inkster, J. A. (1968).Postdecision dissonance at post time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4), 319-323.
  21. Mills, J. (1958). "Changes in moral attitudes following temptation". Journal of Personality 26 (4): 517–531. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1958.tb02349.x.
  22. 1 2 Hart, W.; Albarracín, D.; Eagly, A. H.; Brechan, I.; Lindberg, M. J.; Merrill, L. (2009). "Feeling validated versus being correct: a meta-analysis of selective exposure to information.". Psychological Bulletin 135 (4): 555–588. doi:10.1037/a0015701.
  23. Aronson, E. (1995). The Social Animal. New York: W.H. Freeman and Co.
  24. Lepper, M. R.; Greene, D. (1975). "Turning play into work: Effects of adult surveillance and extrinsic rewards on children's intrinsic motivation" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31: 479–486. doi:10.1037/h0076484.
  25. 1 2 Guzzetti, B.J.; Snyder, T.E.; Glass, G.V.; Gamas, W.S. (1993). "Promoting conceptual change in science: A comparative meta-analysis of instructional interventions from reading education and science education". Reading Research Quarterly 28: 116–159. JSTOR 747886.
  26. Graesser, A. C.; Baggett, W.; Williams, K. (1996). "Question-driven explanatory reasoning". Applied Cognitive Psychology 10: S17–S32. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199611)10:7%3C17::AID-ACP435%3E3.0.CO;2-7.
  27. Cooper, J. (2007). Cognitive dissonance: 50 years of a classic theory. London: Sage Publications.
  28. Cooper, J., & Axsom, D. (1982). Integration of clinical and social psychology. Oxford University Press.
  29. Mendonca, P. J.; Brehm, S. S. (1983). "Effects of choice on behavioral treatment of overweight children". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 1: 343–358. doi:10.1521/jscp.1983.1.4.343.
  30. Cooper, J. (1980). "Reducing fears and increasing attentiveness: The role of dissonance reduction". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47: 452–460. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(80)90064-5.
  31. Axsom, D.; Cooper, J. (1985). "Cognitive dissonance and psychotherapy: The role of effort justification in inducing weight loss". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 21: 149–160. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(85)90012-5.
  32. Stone, J.; Aronson, E.; Crain, A. L.; Winslow, M. P.; Fried, C. B. (1994). "Inducing hypocrisy as a means for encouraging young adults to use condoms". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 20 (1): 116–128. doi:10.1177/0146167294201012.
  33. Fried, C. B.; Aronson, E. (1995). "Hypocrisy, misattribution, and dissonance reduction". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 (9): 925–933. doi:10.1177/0146167295219007.
  34. Son Hing, L. S.; Li, W.; Zanna, M. P. (2002). "Inducing hypocrisy to reduce prejudicial responses among aversive racists". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38: 71–78. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1484.
  35. Fointiat, V. (2004). "I know what I have to do, but…" When hypocrisy leads to behavioral change". Social Behavior and Personality 32: 741–746. doi:10.2224/sbp.2004.32.8.741.
  36. Kataria, M.; Regner, T. (2012). "Honestly, why are you donating money to charity? An experimental study about self-awareness in status-seeking behavior". Jena Economic Research Papers.
  37. Nyborg, K. (2010). "I Don't Want to Hear About it: Rational Ignorance among Duty-Oriented Consumers". Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 79 (3).
  38. Gbadamosi, Ayantunji (January 2009). "Cognitive dissonance: The implicit explication in low-income consumers' shopping behaviour for "low-involvement" grocery products". International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management 37 (12): 1077–1095. doi:10.1108/09590550911005038. Retrieved July 24, 2012.
  39. Mullikin, Lindsey J (2003). "Beyond reference pricing: Understanding consumers' encounters with unexpected prices". Journal of Products & Brand Management 12 (3): 140–153. doi:10.1108/10610420310476906.
  40. 1 2 Heider, F. (1960). The gestalt theory of motivation. Nebraska symposium on motivation, 8, 145-172
  41. Bem, D.J. (1965). "An experimental analysis of self-persuasion". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 1 (3): 199–218. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(65)90026-0.
  42. Bem, D.J. (1967). "Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena" (PDF). Psychological Review 74 (3): 183–200. doi:10.1037/h0024835. PMID 5342882.
  43. Zanna, M.; Cooper, J. (1974). "Dissonance and the pill: An attribution approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 29 (5): 703–709. doi:10.1037/h0036651.
  44. Kiesler, C.A.; Pallak, M.S. (1976). "Arousal properties of dissonance manipulations". Psychological Bulletin 83 (6): 1014–1025. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.83.6.1014.
  45. Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 4, pp. 1–34. New York: Academic Press.
  46. Tedeschi, J.T.; Schlenker, B.R.; Bonoma, T.V. (1971). "Cognitive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public spectacle?". American Psychologist 26 (8): 685–695. doi:10.1037/h0032110.
  47. Coppin, G.; Delplanque, S.; Cayeux, I.; Porcherot, C.; Sander, D. (2010). "I'm no longer torn after choice: How explicit choices implicitly shape preferences of odors". Psychological Science 21 (8): 489–493. doi:10.1177/0956797610364115. PMID 20424088.
  48. Dupuit, J. (1969). On the measurement of the utility of public works. Readings in Welfare
  49. Higgins, F. (1960). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychological review, 94(3), 319.
  50. Cooper, J., & Fazio, R.H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–266). New York: Academic Press.
  51. Harmon-Jones, E.; Brehm, J.W.; Greenberg, J.; Simon, L.; Nelson, D.E. (1996). "Evidence that the production of aversive consequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 (1): 5–16. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.5.
  52. Chen, M.K.; Risen, J.L. (2010). "How choice affects and reflects preferences: Revisiting the free-choice paradigm". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99 (4): 573–594. doi:10.1037/a0020217.
  53. Holden, S. (In press). Do choices affect preferences? Some doubts and new evidence.
  54. 1 2 Izuma, K.; Matsumoto, M.; Murayama, K.; Samejima, K.; Sadato, N.; Matsumoto, K. (2010). "Neural correlates of cognitive dissonance and choice-induced preference change". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A. 107 (51): 22014–22019. doi:10.1073/pnas.1011879108.
  55. Sharot, T.; Velasquez, C. M.; Dolan, R. J. (2010). "Do decisions shape preference? Evidence from blind choice". Psychological Science 21 (9): 1231–1235. doi:10.1177/0956797610379235.
  56. Risen, J.L.; Chen, M.K. (2010). "How to study choice-induced attitude change: Strategies for fixing the free-choice paradigm" (PDF). Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4 (12): 1151–1164. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00323.x.
  57. Beckmann, J; Kuhl, J (1984). "Altering information to gain action control: Functional aspects of human information processing in decision making". Journal of Research in Personality 18: 224–237. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(84)90031-x.
  58. 1 2 Harmon-Jones, E., 1999. Toward an understanding of the motivation underlying dissonance processes: is feeling personally responsible for the production of aversive consequences necessary to cause dissonance effects? In: Harmon-Jones, E., Mills, J., Cognitive Dissonance: Perspectives on a Pivotal Theory in Social Psychology. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 71–99.
  59. Harmon-Jones, E (2000a). "Cognitive dissonance and experienced negative affect: Evidence that dissonance increases experienced negative affect even in the absence of aversive consequences". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26: 1490–1501. doi:10.1177/01461672002612004.
  60. Jones, E. E., Gerard, H. B., 1967. Foundations of social psychology. New York: Wiley.
  61. McGregor, I., Newby-Clark, I. R., Zanna, M. P., 1999. Epistemic discomfort is moderated by simultaneous accessibility of inconsistent elements. In: E. Harmon-Jones, J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 325–353.
  62. 1 2 Van Veen, V.; Krug, M.K.; Schooler, J.W.; Carter, C.S. (2009). "Neural activity predicts attitude change in cognitive dissonance" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience 12 (11): 1469–1474. doi:10.1038/nn.2413.
  63. Sharot, T.; De Martino, B.; Dolan, R.J. (2009). "How choice reveals and shapes expected hedonic outcome" (PDF). Journal of Neuroscience 29 (12): 3760–3765. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4972-08.2009.
  64. Qin, J.; Kimel, S.; Kitayama, S.; Wang, X.; Yang, X.; Han, S. (2011). "How choice modifies preference: Neural correlates of choice justification". NeuroImage 55 (1): 240–246. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.076.
  65. 1 2 3 Jarcho, Johanna M.; Berkman, Elliot T.; Lieberman, Matthew D. (2010). "The neural basis of rationalization: cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making". Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience 6: 460–467. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq054.
  66. Harmon-Jones, E (2003). "Anger and the behavioural approach system". Personality and Individual Differences 35: 995–1005. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00313-6.
  67. Harmon-Jones, E (2004). "Contributions From Research On Anger And Cognitive Dissonance To Understanding The Motivational Functions Of Asymmetrical Frontal Brain Activity". Biological Psychology 67: 51–76. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.003.
  68. Amodio, D.M; Harmon-Jones, E; Devine, P.G; Curtin, J.J; Hartley, S (2004). "A Covert Neural signals for the control of unintentional race bias". Psychological Science 15: 88–93. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502003.x.
  69. Beauvois, J. L., Joule, R. V., 1996. A radical dissonance theory. London: Taylor and Francis.
  70. Egan, L.C.; Santos, L.R.; Bloom, P. (2007). "The origins of cognitive dissonance: Evidence from children and monkeys" (PDF). Psychological Science 18 (11): 978–983. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02012.x.
  71. 1 2 Read, S.J.; Vanman, E.J.; Miller, L.C. (1997). "Connectionism, parallel constraint satisfaction processes, and Gestalt principles: (Re)Introducing cognitive dynamics to social psychology". Personality and Social Psychology Review 1 (1): 26–53. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_3.
  72. Petty, R.E.; Briñol, P.; DeMarree, K.G. (2007). "The Meta-Cognitive Model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement, change, and strength". Social Cognition 25 (5): 657–686. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.657.
  73. Van Overwalle, F.; Jordens, K. (2002). "An adaptive connectionist model of cognitive dissonance". Personality and Social Psychology Review 6 (3): 204–231. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603_6.
  74. Monroe, B.M.; Read, S.J. (2008). "A general connectionist model of attitude structure and change: The ACS (Attitudes as Constraint Satisfaction) Model". Psychological Review 115 (3): 733–759. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.3.733. PMID 18729597.
  75. Van Harreveld, F.; der Pligt, J.; de Liver, Y. (2009). "The agony of ambivalence and ways to resolve it: Introducing the MAID model". Personality and Social Psychology Review 13: 45–61. doi:10.1177/1088868308324518.

Further reading

External links

Videos
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the Tuesday, February 09, 2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.