Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth
The Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY) is a prospective longitudinal survey study of persons (mostly in the United States) identified by scores of 700 or higher on a section of the SAT Reasoning Test before age thirteen. It is one of the longest-running longitudinal studies of gifted youth in world history. Study scholars have used survey data from study participants to advance hypotheses about talent development and occupational preferences.
History
The Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth was founded by Julian Stanley in 1971 at Johns Hopkins University. In 1986, the study headquarters moved to Iowa State University, where Camilla Benbow led the study until 1990. Since that year, the study has been led by Benbow and David Lubinski. In 1998, the study headquarters moved again, this time to Vanderbilt University.
Study ascertainment
SMPY is the longest-running current longitudinal study of gifted children in the United States. Subjects are identified by high scores on the SAT Reasoning Test, which they take at or before the age of thirteen. Eligibility for the study is contingent on scoring at least 700 out of a possible 800 standard score points on the SAT by age thirteen (with prorated eligibility for higher scores up to age thirteen and ten months). Participation in the Talent Search testing that involves children of that age taking the SAT is voluntary.
Participation in SMPY is voluntary for students who attain eligible scores. Although after the first year, Stanley decided to include students with exceptional scores in either the mathematics or verbal sections of the SAT test, for inclusion in what is called the Study of Exceptional Talent,[1] the name SMPY has been retained for the ongoing follow-up surveys.[2] Follow-up surveys were sent to study participants after five, ten, twenty, and thirty-five years.[3] Benbow and Lubinski and their colleagues have used the survey responses to explore individual differences among intellectually able individuals. The study population is analyzed by division into several subgroups.
Findings
The survey responses suggest that the profoundly gifted have different educational needs and accomplish much more in school and work than moderately gifted. Talented males and females also have differing abilities, interests, and lifestyle preferences, although they often express similar levels of intellectual satisfaction and achieve advanced educational credentials at similar rates. The sex differences other investigators have found on the things-people dimension in normative populations have been manifested in education and work, among the adolescents Benbow, Lubinski, and their associates have studied. SMPY has found that talented individuals with marked tilts tend to pursue careers that draw upon their cognitive strengths. Highly able youth with notably stronger mathematical than verbal ability often study and work in science and engineering, whereas adolescents with better scores on the verbal section than the mathematical one frequently went into the humanities, arts, social science, or law. Individuals with comparable mathematical and verbal ability did not follow such clear-cut trajectories, although many males with the "high-flat" ability profile pursued educational and vocational pursuits in science.[4] [5]
References
- ↑ Johns Hopkins University Center for Talented Youth (24 January 2012). "Study of Exceptional Talent". Study of Exceptional Talent website. Baltimore (MD): Johns Hopkins University Center for Talented Youth. Retrieved 16 May 2014.
- ↑ Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (24 January 2012). "Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth". Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth website. Nashville (TN): Vanderbilt University Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth. Retrieved 16 May 2014.
- ↑ Lubinski, David; Benbow, Camilla Persson (2006). "Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth After 35 Years: Uncovering Antecedents for the Development of Math-Science Expertise". Perspectives on Psychological Science (Association for Psychological Science) 1 (4): 316–345. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00019.x. Retrieved 16 May 2014.
- ↑ Benbow, Camilla Persson; Lubinski, David; Shea, Daniel L.; Eftekhari-Sanjani, Hossain (November 2000). "Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability at age 13: Their status 20 years later". Psychological Science 11 (6): 474–80. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00291. PMID 11202492. Retrieved 16 May 2014.
- ↑ Lubinski, David; Webb, Rose Mary; Morelock, Martha J.; Benbow, Camilla Persson (2001). "Top 1 in 10,000: A 10-Year Follow-Up of the Profoundly Gifted". Journal of Applied Psychology 86 (4): 718–729. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.4.718. Retrieved 16 May 2014.
Bibliography
- Bock, Gregory; Ackrill, Kate, eds. (1993). The Origins and Development of High Ability. Ciba Foundation Symposium 178. Chichester: Wiley. p. 119. ISBN 978-0-470-51450-4. Lay summary (28 July 2010).
- Heller, Kurt A.; Mönks, Franz J.; Sternberg, Robert J.; Subotnik, Rena F., eds. (2000). International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Pergamon. p. 318. ISBN 978-0-08-043796-5. Lay summary (6 October 2013).
- Hunt, Earl (2011). Human Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 346. ISBN 978-0-521-70781-7. Lay summary (28 April 2013).
External links
Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth website
Forty Years Later: What Happens to Mathematically Precocious Youth Identified at Age 12? video of 30 April 2014 presentation by David Lubinski at the University of Minnesota department of psychology colloquium series