Peer pressure

For other uses, see Peer pressure (disambiguation).

Peer pressure is influence on a peer group, observers or individual exerts that encourages others to change their attitudes, values, or behaviors to conform to groups. Social groups affected include membership groups, in which individuals are "formally" members (such as political parties and trade unions), or social cliques in which membership is not clearly defined. A person affected by peer pressure may or may not want to belong to these groups. They may also recognise dissociative groups with which they would not wish to associate, and thus they behave adversely concerning that group's behaviors.

In youth

Peers become an important influence on behavior during adolescence, and peer pressure has been called a hallmark of adolescent experience.[1][2] Peer conformity in young people is most pronounced with respect to style, taste, appearance, ideology, and values.[3] Peer pressure is commonly associated with episodes of adolescent risk taking (such as delinquency, drug abuse, sexual behaviors,[4] and reckless driving) because these activities commonly occur in the company of peers.[2] Affiliation with friends who engage in risk behaviors has been shown to be a strong predictor of an adolescent's own behavior.[5] Peer pressure can also have positive effects when youth are pressured by their peers toward positive behavior, such as volunteering for charity [6] or excelling in academics.[7] The importance of peers declines upon entering adulthood.[8]

While socially accepted kids often have the most opportunities and the most positive experiences, research shows that being in the popular crowd may also be a risk factor for mild to moderate deviant behavior. Popular adolescents are the most socialized into their peer groups and thus are vulnerable to peer pressures, such as behaviors usually reserved for those of a greater maturity and understanding. Socially accepted kids are often accepted for the sheer fact that they conform well to the norms of teen culture, good and bad aspects included. Popular adolescents are more strongly associated with their peer groups' likes such as alcohol, tobacco and drugs. Some studies also show that many popular students also make lower grades than less socially accepted kids. This is possibly due to the fact that popular students may spend more time worrying about their social life rather than studying. Although there are a few risk factors correlated with popularity, deviant behavior is often only mild to moderate. Regardless, social acceptance provides more overall protective factors than risk factors.[9]

Substance use and adolescents

Peer pressure is widely recognized as a major contributor to the initiation of substance use, particularly in adolescence.[10] This has been shown across substances, including nicotine,[11] drug use,[12] and alcohol.[13] While this link is well established, mediating factors do exist. For example, parental monitoring is negatively associated with substance use but when there is little monitoring adolescents are more likely to succumb to peer coercion during initiation to substance use but not during the transition from experimental to regular use.[14] Caldwell and colleagues extended this work by finding that peer pressure was a factor leading to heightened risk in the context of social gatherings, little parental monitoring, and if the individual reported themselves as vulnerable to peer pressure.[15] Conversely, some research has observed that peer pressure can be a protective factor against substance use.[16]

Substance use is likely not attributed to peer pressure alone. Evidence of genetic predispositions for substance use exists[17] and some have begun to examine gene x environment interactions for peer influence. In a nationally representative sample, adolescents who had genetic predisposition were more likely to have good friends who were heavy substance users and were furthermore, more likely to be vulnerable to the adverse influence of these friends.[18] Results from specific candidate gene studies have been mixed. For instance, in a study of nicotine use Johnson and colleagues found that peer smoking had a lower effect on nicotine dependence for those with the high risk allele (CHRNA5).[19] This suggests that social contexts do not play the significant role in substance use initiation and maintenance as it may for others and that interventions for these individuals should be developed with this consideration.

Though the impact of peer influence in adolescence has been well established, it was unclear at what age this effect begins to diminish. It is accepted that such peer pressure to use alcohol or illicit substances is less likely to exist in elementary school and very young adolescents given the limited access and exposure. Using the Resistance to Peer Influence Scale, Sumter and colleagues found that resistance to peer pressure grew as age increased in a large study of 10-18 year olds.[20] This study also found that girls were generally more resistant to peer influence than boys, particularly at mid-adolescence (i.e. ages 13–15). The higher vulnerability to peer pressure for teenage boys makes sense given the higher rates of substance use in male teens.[21] For girls, increased and positive parental behaviors (e.g. parental social support, consistent discipline) has been shown to be an important contributor to the ability to resist peer pressure to use substances.[22]

Peer pressure produces a wide array of negative outcomes. Allen and colleagues showed that susceptibility to peer pressure in 13-14 year olds was predictive of not only future response to peer pressure, but also a wider array of functioning.[23] For example, greater depression symptomatology, decreasing popularity, more sexual behavior, and externalizing behavior were greater for more susceptible teens. Of note, substance use was also predicted by peer pressure susceptibility such that greater susceptibility was predictive of greater alcohol and drug use.

Prevention and intervention

Substance use prevention and intervention programs have utilized multiple techniques in order to combat the impact of peer pressure. One major technique is, naturally, peer influence resistance skills.[24][25] The known correlational relationship between substance use and relationships with others that use makes resistance skills a natural treatment target. This type of training is meant to help individuals refuse participation with substance use while maintaining their membership in the peer group. Other interventions include normative education approaches (interventions designed to teach students about the true prevalence rates and acceptability of substance use),[25] education interventions that raise awareness of potential dangers of substance use,[26] alcohol awareness training and classroom behavior management. The literature regarding the efficacy of these approaches, however, is mixed.[25][27][28]

Peer Pressure and Sexual Intercourse

There is evidence supporting that parental attitudes disapproving sex tends to lead toward lower levels of adolescent unplanned pregnancy.[29] These disparities are not due solely to parental disposition but also on communication.

Asch conformity

The Asch conformity experiments were a series of laboratory studies published in the 1950s that demonstrated a surprising degree of conformity to a majority opinion. These are also known as the Asch Paradigm.

Experiments led by Solomon Asch of Swarthmore College asked groups of students to participate in a "sight test." During the sight test, the experimenter displayed two cards- one card depicted three numbered lines of varying length, and the second card depicted one line. Participants were asked which of the three lines corresponded in length to the line from the first card. Participants were instructed to give their answers aloud. However, in reality, all but one of the participants were confederates (i.e., actors planted in the experiment by Asch), and the study was really about how the remaining student would react to the confederates' behavior.

Accordingly on some trials the confederate(s) would answer with an obviously incorrect response. Despite the obvious error, participants felt social pressure to conform, answering incorrectly on 36.8% of trials to maintain conformity.[30] In fact, 76.4% of participants answered incorrectly at least once, a massive difference from the 5% of incorrect responses found in control trials (i.e., when tested alone).

The results of Asch's study, as well as various replications have strong implications for factors that affect peer pressure.

Factors Affecting Likelihood to Conform

A variety of factors identified by Forsyth (2009) have been linked with likelihood to conform to peer pressure.

Responses to Peer Pressure

There are four possible responses to peer pressure:

The Third Wave

The Third Wave was an experiment to demonstrate the appeal of fascism undertaken by history teacher Ron Jones with sophomore high school students attending his Contemporary History as part of a study of Nazi Germany. The experiment took place at Cubberley High School in Palo Alto, California, during the first week of April 1967. Jones, unable to explain to his students how the German populace could claim ignorance of the extermination of the Jewish people, decided to show them instead. Jones started a movement called "The Third Wave" and convinced his students that the movement is to eliminate democracy. The fact that democracy emphasizes individuality was considered as a drawback of democracy, and Jones emphasized this main point of the movement in its motto: "Strength through discipline, strength through community, strength through action, strength through pride". The Third Wave experiment is an example of risk behavior in authoritarian peer pressure situations.[31][32]

As a leadership tool

It is one useful tool in leadership. Instead of direct delegation of tasks and results demanding, employees are in this case, induced into a behavior of self-propelled performance and innovation, by comparison feelings towards their peers. There are several ways peer pressure can be induced in a working environment. Examples include training and team meetings. In training, the team member is in contact with people with comparable roles in other organizations. In team meetings, there is an implicit comparison between every team member, especially if the meeting agenda is to present results and goal status.[33]

Neural mechanisms

Neuroimaging identifies the anterior insula and anterior cingulate as key areas in the brain determining whether people conform in their preferences in regard to its being popular with their peer group.[34]

Explanation

An explanation of how the peer pressure process works, called "the identity shift effect", is introduced by social psychologist, Wendy Treynor, who weaves together Leon Festinger's two seminal social-psychological theories (on cognitive dissonance, which addresses internal conflict, and social comparison, which addresses external conflict) into a unified whole. According to Treynor's original "identity shift effect" hypothesis, the peer pressure process works in the following way: One's state of harmony is disrupted when faced with the threat of external conflict (social rejection) for failing to conform to a group standard. Thus, one conforms to the group standard, but as soon as one does, eliminating this external conflict, internal conflict is introduced (because one has violated one's own standards). To rid oneself of this internal conflict (self-rejection), an "identity shift" is undertaken, where one adopts the group's standards as one's own, thereby eliminating internal conflict (in addition to the formerly eliminated external conflict), returning one once again to a state of harmony. Even though the peer pressure process begins and ends with one in a (conflict-less) state of harmony, as a result of conflict and the conflict resolution process, one leaves with a new identity—a new set of internalized standards.[35]

See also

References

  1. B. B. Brown, "Adolescents' relationships with peers," In: R. M. Lerner & L. Steinburg (Eds.), Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, 2nd ed, New York: Wiley, 2004, p 363-394.
  2. 2.0 2.1 Steinberg, Laurence; Monahan, Kathryn C. (2007). "Age differences in resistance to peer influence". Developmental Psychology 43 (6): 1531–1543. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1531. PMC 2779518. PMID 18020830.
  3. Kevin Durkin, "Peer Pressure", In: Anthony S. R. Manstead and Miles Hewstone (Eds.), The Blackwell Encyclopedia of Social Psychology, 1996.
  4. Cherie, Amsale (2012). "Peer Pressure Is the Prime Driver of Risky Sexual Behaviors among School Adolescents in Addis Ababa, Ethiopia". World Journal of AIDS 02 (03): 159–164. doi:10.4236/wja.2012.23021.
  5. Spear, Hila J.; Kulbok, Pamela A (2001). "Adolescent Health Behaviors and Related Factors: A Review". Public Health Nursing 18 (2): 82–93. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1446.2001.00082.x. PMID 11285102.
  6. Stephanie Hanes, "Teens and volunteering: Altruism or just peer pressure?" The Christian Science Monitor, October 25, 2012.
  7. Kellie B. Gormly, "Peer Pressure -- for students and adults -- can be positive," Pittsburgh Tribune-Review, March 18, 2013.
  8. Brown, B. Bradford; Eicher, Sue Ann; Petrie, Sandra (1986). "The importance of peer group ("crowd") affiliation in adolescence". Journal of Adolescence 9 (1): 73–96. doi:10.1016/S0140-1971(86)80029-X.
  9. Allen, Joseph P.; Porter, Maryfrances R.; McFarland, F. Christy; Marsh, Penny; McElhaney, Kathleen Boykin (2005). "The Two Faces of Adolescents' Success With Peers: Adolescent Popularity, Social Adaptation, and Deviant Behavior". Child Development 76 (3): 747–760. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00875.x. PMC 1551978. PMID 15892790.
  10. Bahr, Stephen J.; Hoffmann, John P.; Yang, Xiaoyan (15 October 2005). "Parental and Peer Influences on the Risk of Adolescent Drug Use". The Journal of Primary Prevention 26 (6): 529–551. doi:10.1007/s10935-005-0014-8.
  11. Urberg, Kathryn A.; Shiang-Jeou, Shyu; Liang, Jersey (1990). "Peer influence in adolescent cigarette smoking". Addictive Behaviors 15 (3): 247–255. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(90)90067-8. PMID 2378284.
  12. Farrell, Albert D.; White, Kamila S. (April 1998). "Peer influences and drug use among urban adolescents: Family structure and parent-adolescent relationship as protective factors.". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 66 (2): 248–258. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.2.248.
  13. Dielman, T. E.; Butchart, A. T.; Shope, J. T. (1993). "Structural Equation Model Tests of Patterns of Family Interaction, Peer Alcohol Use, and Intrapersonal Predictors of Adolescent Alcohol Use and Misuse". Journal of Drug Education 23 (3): 273–316. doi:10.2190/8YXM-K9GB-B8FD-82NQ.
  14. Steinberg, Laurence; Fletcher, Anne; Darling, Nancy (1 June 1994). "Parental Monitoring and Peer Influences on Adolescent Substance Use". Pediatrics 93: 1060. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511527906.016.
  15. Caldwell, Linda; Darling, Nancy (1999). "Leisure Context, Parental Control, and Resistance to Peer Pressure as Predictors of Adolescent Partying and Substance Use: An Ecological Perspective". Journal of Leisure Research 31 (1).
  16. Maxwell, Kimberly A. (August 2002). Journal of Youth and Adolescence 31 (4): 267–277. doi:10.1023/A:1015493316865. Missing or empty |title= (help)
  17. Kendler, Kenneth S.; Prescott, Carol A.; Myers, John; Neale, Michael C. (September 2003). "The Structure of Genetic and Environmental Risk Factors for Common Psychiatric and Substance Use Disorders in Men and Women". Archives of General Psychiatry 60 (9): 929–937. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929.
  18. Harden, K. Paige; Hill, Jennifer E.; Turkheimer, Eric; Emery, Robert E. (27 March 2008). "Gene-Environment Correlation and Interaction in Peer Effects on Adolescent Alcohol and Tobacco Use". Behavior Genetics 38 (4): 339–347. doi:10.1007/s10519-008-9202-7.
  19. Johnson, Eric O.; Chen, Li-Shiun; Breslau, Naomi; Hatsukami, Dorothy; Robbins, Tania; Saccone, Nancy L.; Grucza, Richard A.; Bierut, Laura J. (November 2010). "Peer smoking and the nicotinic receptor genes: an examination of genetic and environmental risks for nicotine dependence". Addiction 105 (11): 2014–2022. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03074.x.
  20. Sumter, Sindy R.; Bokhorst, Caroline L.; Steinberg, Laurence; Westenberg, P. Michiel (August 2009). "The developmental pattern of resistance to peer influence in adolescence: Will the teenager ever be able to resist?". Journal of Adolescence 32 (4): 1009–1021. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.08.010.
  21. Merikangas, Kathleen Ries; He, Jian-ping; Burstein, Marcy; Swanson, Sonja A.; Avenevoli, Shelli; Cui, Lihong; Benjet, Corina; Georgiades, Katholiki; Swendsen, Joel (October 2010). "Lifetime Prevalence of Mental Disorders in U.S. Adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A)". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 49 (10): 980–989. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017.
  22. Marshal, Michael P.; Chassin, Laurie (March 2000). "Peer Influence on Adolescent Alcohol Use: The Moderating Role of Parental Support and Discipline". Applied Developmental Science 4 (2): 80–88. doi:10.1207/S1532480XADS0402_3.
  23. Allen, Joseph P.; Porter, Maryfrances R.; McFarland, F. Christy (2006). "Leaders and followers in adolescent close friendships: Susceptibility to peer influence as a predictor of risky behavior, friendship instability, and depression". Development and Psychopathology 18 (01). doi:10.1017/S0954579406060093. PMC 1557636. PMID 16478557.
  24. Tobler, Nancy (1986). "Meta-analysis of 143 adolescent drug prevention programs: Quantitative outcome results of program participants compared to a control or comparison group". Journal of Drug Issues 16 (4): 537–567.
  25. 25.0 25.1 25.2 Hansen, William B.; Graham, John W. (May 1991). "Preventing alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use among adolescents: Peer pressure resistance training versus establishing conservative norms". Preventive Medicine 20 (3): 414–430. doi:10.1016/0091-7435(91)90039-7.
  26. Foxcroft, David; Ireland, Diana; Lowe, Geoff; Breen, Rosie. "Primary prevention for alcohol misuse in young people". Protocols (Cochrane Drugs and Alcohol Group). doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003024.
  27. Foxcroft, David R; Tsertsvadze, Alexander (March 2012). "Cochrane Review: Universal school-based prevention programs for alcohol misuse in young people". Evidence-Based Child Health: A Cochrane Review Journal 7 (2): 450–575. doi:10.1002/ebch.1829.
  28. Shope, Jean T.; Copeland, Laurel A.; Maharg, Ruth; Dielman, T.E. (August 1996). "Effectiveness of a High School Alcohol Misuse Prevention Program". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 20 (5): 791–798. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb05253.x.
  29. "Influence of Teens' Perceptions of Parental Disapproval and Peer Behaviour on Their Initiation of Sexual Intercourse". The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 14.
  30. (Asch 1952, 1957)
  31. Weinfield, L (1991). Remembering the 3rd Wave. Retrieved March 6, 2010.
  32. Jones, Ron (1972). THE THIRD WAVE. Retrieved March 6, 2010.
  33. Salvador, José (2009). MBA CookBook.
  34. Berns, Gregory S.; Capra, C. Monica; Moore, Sara; Noussair, Charles (2010). "Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music". NeuroImage 49 (3): 2687–2696. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.070. PMC 2818406. PMID 19879365.
  35. Treynor, Wendy (2009). Towards a General Theory of Social Psychology: Understanding Human Cruelty, Human Misery, and, Perhaps, a Remedy (A Theory of the Socialization Process). Redondo Beach: Euphoria Press. ISBN 0982302878.