Diagnostic substitution
Diagnostic substitution is a phenomenon in which one label for a condition becomes replaced with another, causing an apparent decrease in the rate of the first condition and increase in the rate of the second.[1]
Autism
The best-known example is that of the increasing rates of autism in developed countries such as the United States, which some studies suggest is at least partly a result of people substituting diagnoses of autism for mental retardation and learning disabilities.[2][3][4] While a pilot study by the MIND Institute published in 2002 concluded that "There is no evidence that loosening in diagnostic criteria contributed to an increase in the number of children with autism,"[5] this study used data from the California Department of Developmental Services database, which, according to a study by Paul Shattuck, is unreliable because "...the administrative prevalence figures for most states are well below epidemiological estimates."[2] With regard to the role of diagnostic substitution in the increase in reported cases of autism, Dorothy Bishop has said, "This could be in part because of new conceptualisations of autism, but may also be fuelled by strategic considerations: resources for children with ASD tend to be much better than those for children with other related conditions, such as language impairment or intellectual handicaps, so this diagnosis may be preferred."[6][7] Bishop has herself published a small study concluding that people who would now be diagnosed as autistic would, in the past, have been diagnosed with developmental language disorder.[8] Emily Willingham has noted that at the time autism was first reported by Leo Kanner in 1943, while autistic people existed before that time, they had historically been referred to as insane, schizophrenic, mentally retarded, or language impaired.[9]
References
- ↑ Shattuck, Paul. "Diagnostic Substitution". Springer Reference. Springer Science+Business Media. Retrieved 6 March 2014.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 Shattuck, P. T. (2006). "The Contribution of Diagnostic Substitution to the Growing Administrative Prevalence of Autism in US Special Education". Pediatrics 117 (4): 1028–1037. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-1516. PMID 16585296.
- ↑ Coo, H.; Ouellette-Kuntz, H. L. N.; Lloyd, J. E. V.; Kasmara, L.; Holden, J. J. A.; Lewis, M. E. S. (2007). "Trends in Autism Prevalence: Diagnostic Substitution Revisited". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38 (6): 1036–1046. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0478-x. PMID 17975721.
- ↑ King, M.; Bearman, P. (2009). "Diagnostic change and the increased prevalence of autism". International Journal of Epidemiology 38 (5): 1224–1234. doi:10.1093/ije/dyp261. PMC 2800781. PMID 19737791.
- ↑ Byrd RS; Sage AC; Keyzer J et al. (2002). "Report to the legislature on the principal findings of the epidemiology of autism in California: a comprehensive pilot study" (PDF). M.I.N.D. Institute. Archived from the original on 2008-04-14.
- ↑ Bishop, Dorothy (4 June 2012). "The ‘autism epidemic’ and diagnostic substitution". Deevybee.blogspot.co.uk. Retrieved 9 October 2013.
- ↑ Fung, Brian (4 June 2012). "You say 'Semantic-Pragmatic Disorder,' I Say 'Autism'". The Atlantic. Retrieved 9 October 2013.
- ↑ Bishop, D. V. M.; Whitehouse, A. J. O.; Watt, H. J.; Line, E. A. (2008). "Autism and diagnostic substitution: Evidence from a study of adults with a history of developmental language disorder". Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 50 (5): 341–5. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.02057.x. PMID 18384386.
- ↑ Rothman, Josh (25 July 2012). "Autism: Not Actually on the Rise". Boston.com. Retrieved 9 October 2013.