Attentional retraining
Psychology |
---|
Basic types |
Applied psychology |
Lists |
|
[1]Attentional retraining is the retraining of automatic attentional processes. The method of retraining varies but has typically employed computerized training programs.[2][3] The term originally indicated retraining of attention to rehabilitate individuals after a brain injury who suffered from neurological disorders of attention including hemineglect, perseveration, limited attention span, and even ADHD. However, in more recent research and clinical applications attentional retraining has also been applied as a type of cognitive bias modification.[4] In this application, attentional retraining refers to the retraining of automatic attentional biases that have been observed in high levels of anxiety.
Neuropsychological rehabilitation applications
Computerized approaches to cognitive rehabilitation rose out of the recreational use of video games in the 1970s and the rise of the personal computer in the 1980s. The increased availability of personal computers and accessible programming languages allowed for researchers and clinicians to begin experimenting with computerized cognitive training.[5] Most tasks consisted of simple and repetitive training tasks that would increase in difficulty over time. In one such task participants must sit and observe on screen randomly presented numbers and push a buzzer when they see a specific digit - say, 3.[6] Similar tasks were developed and administered to individuals demonstrating neuropsychological impairment in areas of attentional processing. The theorized mechanism of action in this approach to cognitive retraining rests on the ability of the generally trained task (recognizing and responding to numbers presented on screen) can generalize to attentional processes employed in every-day life.
Effectiveness in brain injury
In a 2001 meta-analysis of outcomes in attention rehabilitation after brain injury found the authors surveyed 30 studies with 359 patients. In this most recent meta-analysis of outcomes the authors found that there were significant and large effect sizes (d statistic) from pre-training to post-training. However, this large effect size was only found for studies without a control group. When the authors analyzed those pre/post studies with a control group the authors found much smaller effect sizes.[7] This finding may reflect the natural improvement seen over time after brain injury.
Cognitive bias modification applications
Attention Training for anxiety is a form of Applied Cognitive Processing Therapy (ACPT) and is also referred to in the scientific literature as Cognitive Bias Modification therapy for attention. CBM therapies (CBMT) also include applied cognitive processing therapies for attention bias, interpretation bias and imagery.
Attention Bias
The attentional bias[8] is the tendency of certain salient cues in a person's environment to preferentially draw and/or hold the person's attention. For example, individuals with anxiety disorders demonstrate an automatic attentional bias towards threatening cues in their environment and drug users and addicts demonstrate an automatic attentional bias towards drug related cues in their environment.[9][10]
The rise of research in cognitive bias modification has led to the recent publication of a special issue of the Journal of Abnormal Psychology focusing on the methods and technologies used for cognitive bias modification in psychopathology.[11] Attentional retraining as cognitive bias modification is predicated on the observed attentional bias evident in psychopathology. The most common task used to retrain attention in anxiety is the dot-probe task developed originally by Macleod et al. (1986). In this task two stimuli are briefly presented on screen. One of the stimuli is emotionally salient (a drug cue or threat cue) and the other is neutral. Stimuli (usually words or images) are presented for about 500 milliseconds and then one of the stimuli is replaced by a probe to which the participant must respond. Required responses are usually to indicate what side of the screen the probe is on or to indicate which direction the probe (an arrow in this case) is pointing. The attentional bias is indicated by the difference in reaction time to the probe after it replaces a salient cue versus the reaction time to respond to a cue that replaces the neutral cue. Usually, the anxious participant will be faster in responding to probes replacing salient stimuli than those replacing neutral stimuli. This suggests their attention was drawn preferentially by the previously presented salient stimuli.
Attentional retraining attempts to retrain this automatic attentional process by using the dot-probe task and having the probe replace the neutral stimuli 100% of the time. Thus, the participant learns an implicit if-then rule: if both salient and neutral stimuli are present, then attend preferentially to the neutral stimuli.[12] Therefore, if attentional biases have a causal role on the maintenance of anxiety or drug addictions then lowering the attentional biases should therefore lower feelings of anxiety amongst the anxious and craving and promote abstinence amongst the drug addicted.
Effectiveness of CBMT for Anxiety
Effectiveness of attentional retraining has varied in both the anxiety and addictions literature.
In anxiety, cognitive bias modification therapy results have been much more promising and clear with several studies showing reductions in anxiety amongst the experimental group compared to control.[13][14][15][16] Studies have thus far shown a greater effect of CBMT on anxiety when trials are conducted in a clinic[17] than when conducted at home.[18]
How attention bias modification works. It has been found that attention training works through an improvement in the ability to disengage attention from threatening cues.[19][20] Moreover, it has also been evidenced that attention bias modification is related to changes in activation of the prefrontal cortex to emotional stimuli.[21]
Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction
Two large scale recent studies have shown that CBM training with an approach/avoid task in alcohol dependent individuals can reduce relapse rates at one year up to 10%.[22]
In an earlier study of attentional retraining in alcohol drinking attentional retraining altered attentional biases and altered the amount of alcohol subsequently consumed after training compared to a control group.[23]
References
- ↑ Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., . . . Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry, 68(11), 982-990. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021
- ↑ Macleod, C., Soong, L., Rutherford, E. M., & Campbell, L. W. (2007). Internet-delivered assessment and manipulation of anxiety-linked attentional bias: Validation of a free-access attentional probe software package. Behaviour Research Methods, 39(3), 533-538.
- ↑ Gray, J., Robertson, I., Pentland, B., & Anderson, S. (1992). Microcomputer-based attentional retraining after brain damage: A randomised group controlled trial. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 2(2), 97-115. DOI: http://dx.doi.org.lrc1.usuhs.edu/10.1080/09602019208401399
- ↑ Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., . . . Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry, 68(11), 982-990. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021
- ↑ Lynch, B. (2002). Historical review of computer-assisted cognitive retraining. The Journal of head trauma rehabilitation, 17(5), 446.
- ↑ Sohlberg, M., & Mateer, C. (1987). Effectiveness of an attention-training program. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9(2), 117-130.
- ↑ Park, N., & Ingles, J. (2001). Effectiveness of attention rehabilitation after an acquired brain injury: A meta-analysis. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY-NEW YORK-, 15(2), 199-210.
- ↑ Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van, I. M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull, 133(1), 1-24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
- ↑ MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15-20.
- ↑ Waters, A. J., Shiffman, S., Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (2003a). Attentional shifts to smoking cues in smokers. Addiction, 98(10), 1409-1417.
- ↑ Journal of Abnormal Psychology (2009), 118(1)
- ↑ Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 16(3), 331-354.
- ↑ MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107-123.
- ↑ See, J., MacLeod, C., & Bridle, R. (2009). The reduction of vulnerability through the modification of attentional bias: A real world study using a home-based cognitive bias modification procedure. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 65 - 75
- ↑ Amir, N., Beard, C., Burns, M., & Bomyea, J. (2009). Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of abnormal psychology, 118(1), 28-33.
- ↑ Heeren, A., Reese, H. E., McNally, R. J., & Philippot, P. (2012). Attention training toward and away from treat in social phobia: Effects on behavioural, subjective, and physiological measures of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 30-39.
- ↑ Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., . . . Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry, 68(11), 982-990. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021
- ↑ Boettcher J, Leek L, Matson L, Holmes, E. A., Browning M, MacLeod C, . . . P, C. Internet-based attention modification for social anxiety: a randomised controlled comparison of training towards negative and training towards positive cues (SUBMITTED).
- ↑ Heeren, A., Lievens, L., & Philippot, P. (2011). How does attention training work in social phobia: Disengagement from threat or reengagement to non-threat? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 1108-1115.
- ↑ Amir, N., Weber, G., Beard, C., Bomyea, J., & Taylor, C. T. (2008). The effects of a single-session attention modification program on response to a public-speaking challenge in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 860-868.
- ↑ Browning, M., Holmes, E. A., Murphy, S. E., Goodwin, G. M., & Harmer, C. J. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex mediates the cognitive modification of attentional bias. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 919-925.
- ↑ Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(0), 38-51. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.002
- ↑ Schoenmakers, T., Wiers, R. W., Jones, B. T., Bruce, G., & Jansen, A. T. M. (2007). Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addiction, 102(3), 399-405.