Adaptive Toolbox
The Adaptive toolbox is the ability of an institution or individual to make expedient decisions. The concept was originated by Gerd Gigerenzer. The adaptive toolbox employs the theory of ecological rationality.[1] Businesses or individuals employ the adaptive toolbox in order to handle situations of uncertainty involving limited time, computational resources and information. The content of the adaptive toolbox is shaped by evolution, learning, and culture for specific domains of inference and reasoning, as well as changes across the life stages .[2] In short, it is a decision-making method that uses an individual's past experiences and problem solving skills to make decisions in an unfamiliar or high-stress environment. Heuristics is another term for these decision-making models.
The adaptive toolbox consists of:
- The collection of elements -- such as search rules, stopping rules, and decision rules for constructing heuristics.
- Core mental capacities that building blocks exploit -- such as recognition memory, depth perception, frequency monitoring, object tracking, and the ability to imitate.
- A specific group of rules or heuristics rather than a general-purpose decision-making algorithm. These heuristics are fast, frugal, and computationally cheap, but are less consistent, coherent, and general.[3]
Common examples include:[3][4]
- a) Recognition-based heuristics (e.g. recognition heuristic, fluency heuristic)
- b) One-reason decision-making (e.g. Take-the-best, Fast and Frugal Trees)
- c) Trade-off heuristics (e.g. 1/N, Tallying)
- d) Satisficing heuristics
- e) Social heuristics (e.g. tit for tat, imitate-the-majority, imitate-the-successful, default heuristic, social circle heuristic,[5] averaging, choosing[6]).
The extent to which humans, and other species, share heuristics depends on the extent to which those humans experience the same adaptive problems, environmental structures, and core capacities. For example, "while the absence of language production from the adaptive toolbox of other animals means they cannot use name recognition to make inferences about their world, some animal species can use other capacities such as taste and smell recognition as input for the recognition heuristic."[7]
The selection of heuristics
The assumption that individuals are equipped with a repertoire of heuristics raises the question of how they select strategies in a given context. Scholars have proposed two main methods to explain how individuals select strategies from the adaptive toolbox:
- the cognitive niches approach;[8] and
- the strategy selection learning theory.[9]
According to the idea of cognitive niches, environmental structure, strategy, and cognitive capacity limit the application of specific heuristics.[8] The result is cognitive niches for different heuristics. For example, when purchasing a mobile phone, a consumer relying on recognition heuristics will choose the familiar brand. However, unfamiliar brands force the consumer to compare all the features, weighing each model and using tallying, or, if lacking time or cognitive capacity, make use of a few important features to compare the two models using take-the-best.
The strategy selection learning theory, in contrast, argues that people select appropriate strategies based on learning. It assumes that individuals form subjective expectations for the strategies they have, select strategies proportionally to these expectations, and update their expectations after the use of the selected strategy.[9] For example, in the case of selecting a strategy for choosing among different options during the purchase of a mobile phone, the strategy selection learning theory proposes that individuals first assess how well each of the available strategies would perform in terms of making the right decision. Then, based on this judgment, they would use the strategy with the best expected outcome. After using a particular strategy to make the purchase of the mobile phone, the choice outcome is evaluated and expectations about the performance of that executed strategy are updated and reinforced as an information for future purchase decisions.
Alternative views
This concept departs from the idea of a single strategy being universally superior as put forward by Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz. Leibniz[10] proposed to replace all reasoning with a universal logical language, the Universal Characteristic. "The multitude of simple concepts constituting Leibniz’s alphabet of human thought were all to be operated on by a single general-purpose tool such as probability theory”.[3] Today, a number of approaches exist that assume a universal strategy: for example rational choice theory, the Bayesian approach to cognition,[11] Parallel constraint satisfaction processes (PCS),[12] sequential-sampling process models such as the adaptive spanner perspective[5] and decision field theory.[13]
See also
- Heuristics
- Heuristics in judgment and decision making
- Ecological rationality
- Decision making
- Gerd Gigerenzer
References
- ↑ Gigerenzer & Selten (2001) Bounded Rationality: The adaptive toolbox, MIT Press
- ↑ Gigerenzer (2003). The adaptive toolbox and life span development: Common questions? In: Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology', U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.). Boston: Kluwer
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 3.2 Todd, Peter; Gigerenzer, Gerd (2000). "Precis of Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5): 727–780. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003447
- ↑ Hertwig, Ralph; Herzog, Stefan (2009). "Fast and Frugal Heuristics: Tools of Social Rationality". Social Cognition 27 (5): 661–698. doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.5.661
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 Newell, B. R. (2005). "Re-visions of rationality?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (1): 11–15. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.005. PMID 15639435.
- ↑ Soll, J. B.; Larrick, R. P. (2009). "Strategies for revising judgment: How (and how well) people use others' opinions". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35 (3): 780. doi:10.1037/a0015145.
- ↑ Todd & Gigerenzer (2012) 'What is ecological rationality?. In: Ecological Rationality, Ed: P. M. Todd, G. Gigerenzer & the ABC Research Group. ,OUP. ISBN 0195315448 ISBN 978-0195315448
- ↑ 8.0 8.1 Marewski, J. N.; Schooler, L. J. (2011). "Cognitive niches: An ecological model of strategy selection". Psychological Review 118 (3): 393–437. doi:10.1037/a0024143. PMID 21744978.
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 Rieskamp, J. R.; Otto, P. E. (2006). "SSL: A Theory of How People Learn to Select Strategies". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135 (2): 207. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.2.207.
- ↑ Leibniz, Gottfried (1995) 'Toward a universal characteristic. In: Leibniz: Selections, Ed: P.P. Wiener. ,Scribner's Sons. ISBN 068412551X ISBN 978-0684125510
- ↑ Jones, M.; Love, B. C. (2011). "Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (4): 169–188; disuccsion 188–231. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10003134. PMID 21864419.
- ↑ Glöckner, Andreas; Betsch, Tilmann (2008). "Modeling option and strategy choices with connectionist networks: Towards an integrative model of automatic and deliberate decision making". Judgment and Decision Making 3: 215–228
- ↑ Busemeyer, Jerome; Townsend, James (1993). "Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment". Psychological Review 100 (3): 432–459. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.432. PMID 8356185