Obviative

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Obviate (abbreviated OBV) third person is a grammatical person marking that distinguishes a non-salient (obviative) third person referent from a more salient (proximate) third person referent in a given discourse context. The obviative is sometimes referred to as the "fourth person".[1]

Geography

North America

Obviate/proximate distinctions are common in some indigenous language families in northern North America. Algonquian languages are perhaps best known for obviation, but the feature also occurs in some Salishan languages and in the language isolate Kutenai, as well as in the more southern Keresan languages.[2]

Africa

Obviative markers are used in some Nilo-Saharan and Niger–Congo languages.[3]

Elsewhere

Obviation has also been attested in the Northeast Caucasian Ingush language.[4]

Cross-linguistic patterns

  • Where animacy is involved, animate noun phrases tend to be proximate, while inanimate noun phrases tend to be obviative.
  • Possessors are frequently obligatorily proximate and possessees are thus obligatory obviative.
  • Obviation is most common in head-marking languages since the obviative is useful in disambiguating otherwise unmarked nominals.[4]
  • The obviative referent seems to always be the marked form, while the proximate is unmarked.
  • Obviative marking tends to apply only to the third person, though it has been attested in the second person in a handful of Nilo-Saharan languages.[3]
  • Proximate/Obviative assignments are preserved throughout clauses and are also often constant over longer discourse segments.[2]

Notable language-specific examples

Ojibwe

The following is a typical example of obviate/proximate morphology in the Eastern dialect of the Algonquian Ojibwe language, in which the obviative is marked on nouns and demonstratives and reflected in pronominal verb affixes:

Maabadashshkinwewgii-bwaadaawii-bi-yaanidmyagi-nishnaabenwaa-bi-nsigwaajin
maabadashoshkinaweo-gii-bawaad-am-nwii-bi-ayaa-ini-dmayagi-nishanaabe-anx-wii-bi-nis-igo-waa-d-in
thisEMPyoung.man3-PAST-dream-3INAN-OBVFUT-coming-be.at-OBV-3foreign-people-OBVREL-FUT-coming-kill-INV-3-OBV

'Then this (PROX) young man (PROX) dreamed (PROX) that foreigners (OBV) would come (OBV) to kill (OBV) them (PROX).'

Note that this example shows that the proximate referent need not necessarily be the subject of a clause.[2]

Potawatomi

The Algonquian Potawatomi language is notable for having two "degrees" of obviation. As is seen in the following example, a "further obviative" referent deemed even less salient than the obviative referent can be marked by reduplication of the obviative suffix:

waposowaposo-nwaposo-n-un
rabbitrabbit-OBVrabbit-OBV-OBV
/proximate//obviative//further obviative/[5]

Ingush

Obviation in the Ingush language, a heavily dependent-marking language, is an exception to the generalization that the obviative occurs in head-marking languages. Obviation is not overtly marked in Ingush, but is implied by the fact that certain constructions are only possible when one referent has salience over another.

For example, if a non-subject-referent has salience over the subject and precedes the other co-referent, reflexivization (normally used only when there is a coreferent to the subject) is possible. This is shown in the example below where the non-subject-referent appears to have salience over the subject:

Muusaajnashiizhwaliit'y-weaxar
Musa-DAT3S-RFL-GENdogon-bark-WITNESSED PAST

'Musa's dog barked at him.'

If the subject is salient ("proximate"), on the other hand, the subject's possessor may not antecede the third person object, and the possession must be indirectly implicated as follows:

Muusaasiesaguoliex
Musawife-ERGseek

'Musa's wife is looking for him.' (Lit. 'The wife is looking for Musa.')[4]

References

  1. Kibort, Anna. "Person." Grammatical Features. 7 January 2008. Retrieved on 2009-10-25.
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 Mithun, Marianne. The languages of Native North America. 76-68.
  3. 3.0 3.1 Gregersen, Edgar A. Language in Africa: an introductory survey. New York: Gordon and Breach Science Publishers Inc, 1977. 51-52.
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 "The Scientific Interest of Ingush - Section 5, Obviation" University of California, Berkeley (Unpublished). Retrieved on 2009-10-29.
  5. Schlenker, Philippe. Propositional Attitudes and Indexicality: A Cross-Categorial Approach. University of Southern California, 1994. 44-45.
7. Aissen, Judith. 1997. On the syntax of obviation. Language 73:4.705-50.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike; additional terms may apply for the media files.