Linguistic intelligence
Linguistic Intelligence is a part of Howard Gardner's multiple intelligence theory that deals with an individual's ability to understand both spoken and written language, as well as their ability to speak and write themselves. In a practical sense, linguistic intelligence is the extent to which an individual can use language, both written and verbal, to achieve goals.[1] In addition to this, high linguistic intelligence has been linked to improved problem solving, as well as to increased abstract reasoning.[2]
In many cases, only the verbal aspects are taken into consideration. This is usually referred to as verbal intelligence or verbal fluency, and is commonly a reflection of an individual's overall linguistic intelligence.
Physiologic and functional overview
In order to understand linguistic intelligence it is important to understand the mechanisms that control speech and language. These mechanisms can be broken down into four major groups: speech generation (talking), speech comprehension (hearing), writing generation (writing), and writing comprehension (reading).
Spoken language
Generation
Speech production is process by which a thought in the brain is converted into an understandable auditory form.[3][4][5] This is a multistage mechanism that involves many different areas of the brain. The first stage is planning, where the brain constructs words and sentences that turn the thought into an understandable form.[3] This occurs primarily in the inferior frontal cortex, specifically in an area known as Broca's area.[4][5][6] Next, the brain must plan how to physically create the sounds necessary for speech by linking the planned speech with known sounds, or phonemes. While the location of these associations is not known, it is known that the supplementary motor area plays a key role in this step.[3][7] Finally, the brain must signal for the words to actually be spoken. This is carried out by the premotor cortex and the motor cortex.[7]
In most cases, speech production is controlled by the left hemisphere. In a series of studies, Wilder Penfield, among others, probed the brains of both right-handed (generally left-hemisphere dominant) and left-handed (generally right-hemisphere dominant) patients. They discovered that, regardless of handedness, the left hemisphere was almost always the speech controlling side. However, it has been discovered that in cases of neural stress (hemorrhage, stroke, etc.) the right hemisphere has the ability to take control of speech functions.[8]
Comprehension
Verbal Comprehension is a fairly complex process, and it is not fully understood. From various studies and experiments, it has been found that the superior temporal sulcus activates when hearing human speech, and that speech processing seems to occur within Wernicke's area.[5][7]
Auditory feedback and feedforward
Hearing plays an important part in both speech generation and comprehension. When speaking, the person can hear their speech, and the brain uses what it hears as a feedback mechanism to fix speech errors.[9] If a single feedback correction occurs multiple times, the brain will begin to incorporate the correction to all future speech, making it a feed forward mechanism.[9] This is apparent in some deaf patients. Deafness, as well as other, smaller deficiencies in hearing, can greatly affect one's ability to comprehend spoken language, as well as to speak it.[10] However, if the patient loses hearing ability later in life, most can still maintain a normal level of verbal intelligence. This is thought to be because of the brain's feed forward mechanism still helping to fix speech errors, even in the absence of auditory feedback.[9]
Written language
Generation
Generation of written language is thought to be closely related to speech generation, relying on Broca's area for early processing and on the inferior frontal gyrus for semantic processing.[5][7] However, writing differs in two major ways. First, instead of relating the thought to sounds, the brain must relate the thought to symbols or letters, and second, the motor cortex activates a different set of muscles to write, than when speaking.[7]
Comprehension
Written comprehension, similar to spoken comprehension, seems to occur primarily in Wernicke's area.[7] However, instead of using the auditory system to gain language input, written comprehension relies on the visual system.
Genetic links
While the capabilities of the physical structures used are large factors in determining linguistic intelligence, there have been several genes that have been linked to individual linguistic ability.[11] The NRXN1 gene has been linked to general language ability, and mutations of this gene has been shown to cause major issues to overall linguistic intelligence.[11] The CNTNAP2 gene is believed to affect language development and performance, and mutations in this gene is thought to be involved in autism spectrum disorders.[11] PCDH11 has been linked to language capacity, and it is believed to be one of the factors that accounts for the variation in linguistic intelligence.[11]
Measurement and testing
In general, it is difficult to test for linguistic intelligence as a whole, therefore verbal intelligence is often measured instead. This is done using various types of verbal fluency tests.[4][6][12]
Verbal fluency tests
- Semantic Fluency Test - Subjects are asked to produce words in groups, such as animals, kitchen tools, fruits, etc. This type of test focuses on the subject's ability to generate words that have meaning to them. This test has been found to be sensitive to age.[12]
- Formal Fluency Test - Subjects are asked to produce words given specific letter-based rules. This test has been found to be sensitive to education level.[12]
- Verb Fluency Test - Subjects are asked to list verbs. Subjects are then tested on their ability to use listed verbs.[12]
- Verbal Reproduction Test - Subjects are asked to listen to a monologue. They are then asked to repeat the monologue, and the subject is scored based on the number of words and lemmas used from the original monologue.[1]
Verbal fluency in children
In one series of tests, it was shown that when children were given verbal fluency tests, a larger portion of their cortex activated compared to adults, as well as activation of both the left and right hemispheres. This is most likely due to the high plasticity of newly developing brains.[13]
Possible conflict
Recently, a study was done showing that verbal fluency test results can differ depending on the mental focus of the subject. In this study, mental focus on physical speech production mechanisms caused speech production times to suffer, whereas mental focus on auditory feedback improved these times.[14]
Disorders affecting linguistic intelligence
Since linguistic intelligence is based on several complex skills, there are many disorders and injuries that can affect an individual's linguistic intelligence.
Injuries
Damage and injury in the brain can severely lower ones ability to communicate, and therefore lower ones linguistic intelligence. Common forms of major damage are strokes, concussions, brain tumors, viral/bacterial damage, and drug-related damage. The three major linguistic disorders that result from these injuries are aphasia, alexia, and agraphia.[7] Aphasia is the inability to speak, and can be caused by damage to Broca's area or the motor cortex.[7] Alexia is the inability to read, which can arise from damage to Wernicke's area, among other places.[7] Agraphia is the inability to write which can also arise from damage to Broca's area or the motor cortex.[7] In addition, damage to large areas of the brain can result in any combinations of these disorders, as well as a loss of other abilities.[7]
Pure language disorders
There are several disorders that primarily affect only language skills. Three major pure language disorders are Developmental verbal dyspraxia, specific language impairment, and stuttering.[11] Developmental verbal dyspraxia (DVD) is a disorder where children have errors in consonant and vowel production.[11] Specific language impairment (SLI) is a disorder where the patient has a lack of language acquisition skills, despite a seemingly normal intelligence level in other areas.[11] Stuttering is a fairly common disorder where speech flow is interrupted by involuntary repetitions of syllables.[11]
Other disorders affecting language
Some disorders cause a wide array of affects, and language impairment is merely one of many possible symptoms. The two major disorders of this type are the autism spectrum disorders and epilepsy.[11] Autism and other autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are disorders in which the patient suffers from decreased social skills and lowered mental flexibility. As a result, many patients suffering ASDs also have language problems, arising from both the lack of social interaction and lowered mental flexibility.[11] Epilepsy is a disorder where electrical malfunctions or mis-communications in the brain cause seizures, leading to muscle spasms and activation of other organs and systems of the body. Over time, epilepsy can lead to cognitive and behavioral decay. This mental decay can eventually lead to a loss of language and communication skills.[11]
See also
- Outline of human intelligence
- Language
- Language acquisition
- Sentence processing
- Auditory processing disorder
- Speech-language pathology
- Theory of multiple intelligences
- Verbal fluency test
- Speech
- Reading education
- Verbal IQ in WAIS-III
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 Fernandez-Martinez, Fernando; Kseniya Zablotskaya, Wolfgang Minker (Aug 2012). "Text categorization methods for automatic estimation of verbal intelligence". Expert Systems with Applications 39 (10): 9807–9820.
- ↑ Luwel, Koen; Ageliki Foustana, Patrick Onghena, Lieven Verschaffel (Apr 2013). "The role of verbal and performance intelligence in children's strategy selection and execution". Learning and Individual Differences 24: 134–138.
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 3.2 Bohland, Jason; Daniel Bullock, Frank Guenther (Jul 2010). "Neural Representations and Mechanisms for the Performance of Simple Speech Sequences". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 (7): 1504–1529.
- ↑ 4.0 4.1 4.2 Dan, Haruka; Sano, Kyutoku, Oguro, Yokota, Tsuzuki, Watanabe (Aug 2013). "Language-specific cortical activation patterns for verbal fluency tasks in Japanese as assessed by multichannel functional near-infrared spectroscopy". Brain and Language 126 (2): 208–216.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 Rodd, J.M.; M.H. Davis, I.S. Johnsrude (Aug 2005). "The neural mechanisms of speech comprehension: fMRI studies of semantic ambiguity". Cerebral Cortex 15 (8): 1261–1269.
- ↑ 6.0 6.1 Konrad, Andreas; Goran Vucurevic, Francesco Musso, Georg Winterer (Apr 2012). "VBM-DTI Correlates of Verbal Intelligence: A Potential Link to Broca's area". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 (4): 888–895.
- ↑ 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 7.4 7.5 7.6 7.7 7.8 7.9 7.10 Penfield, Wilder (1959). Speech and Brain-Mechanism. Atheneum.
- ↑ Doidge, Norman (2007). The Brain That Changes Itself: Stories of Personal Triumph from the Frontiers of Brain Science. Penguin Books.
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 9.2 Perkell, Joseph (Sep 2012). "Movement goals and feedback and feed forward control mechanisms in speech production". Journal of Neurolinguistics 25 (5): 382–407.
- ↑ Tourville, Jason; Kevin Reilly, Frank Guenther (1 February 2008). "Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech". Neuroimage 39 (3): 1429–1443.
- ↑ 11.0 11.1 11.2 11.3 11.4 11.5 11.6 11.7 11.8 11.9 11.10 Szalontai, Adam; Katalin Csiszar (September 2013). "Genetic insights into the functional elements of language". Human Genetics 132 (9): 959–986.
- ↑ 12.0 12.1 12.2 12.3 12.4 12.5 Casals-Coll, M.; Sanchez-Benavides, Quintana, Manero, Rognoni, Calvo, Palomo, Aranciva, Tamayo, Pena-Casanova (Jan–Feb 2013). "Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): Normative data: norms for verbal fluency tests". Neurologia 28 (1): 33–40.
- ↑ Gaillard, W.D.; Hertz-Pannier, Mott, Barnett, LeBihan, Theodore (Jan 2000). "Functional anatomy of cognitive development - fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults". Neurology 54 (1): 180–185.
- ↑ Lisman, Amanda; Neeraja, Sadagopan (May–Jun 2013). "Focus of attention and speech motor performance". Journal of Communication Disorders 46 (3): 281–293.
|