Taste

Taste (also called smatch or gustation; adjectival form: gustatory) is one of the traditional five senses. It refers to the ability to detect the flavor of substances such as food, certain minerals, and poisons, etc.

Humans receive tastes through sensory organs called taste buds,[1] or gustatory calyculi, concentrated on the top of the tongue.[2]

The sensation of taste can be categorized into five basic tastes: sweet, bitterness, sour, salty, and umami. The recognition and awareness of umami is a relatively recent development in Western cuisine.[3] "Umami" is originally the Japanese word for "good taste". Not surprisingly, it is characteristic of many oriental dishes.[4] The MSG amino acid produces a strong umami taste.[5]

As taste senses both harmful and beneficial things, all basic tastes are classified as either appetitive or aversive, depending upon the effect the things they sense have on our bodies.[6] Sweetness helps to identify energy-rich foods, while bitterness serves as a warning sign of poisons.[7]

The basic tastes contribute only partially to the sensation and flavour of food in the mouth — other factors include smell,[1] detected by the olfactory epithelium of the nose;[8] texture,[9] detected through a variety of mechanoreceptors, muscle nerves, etc.;[10] temperature, detected by thermoreceptors; and "coolness" (such as of menthol) and "hotness" (piquance), through chemesthesis.

Contents

Introduction

As one of the senses, taste is an essential part of daily life.

History

In The West, Aristotle, who postulated c. 350 BCE[11] that the two most basic tastes were sweet and bitter,[12] was one of the first to develop a list of basic tastes.[13]

Ayurveda, an ancient Indian healing science, has its own tradition of basic tastes, including: astringent, bitter, pungent, salty, sour, and sweet.[14][15]

Recent discoveries

The receptors for the basic tastes of bitter, sweet and umami have been identified. They are G protein-coupled receptors.[16] The cells that detect sour have been identified as a subpopulation that express the protein PKD2L1. The responses are mediated by an influx of protons into the cells but the receptor for sour is still unknown . The receptor for amiloride-sensitive attractive salty taste in mice has been shown to be a sodium channel.[17] There is some evidence for a sixth taste that senses fatty substances.[18]

Taste-map myth

Despite a common misconception that different sections of the tongue specialized in different tastes, all taste sensations come from all regions of the tongue.[19]

Basic tastes

For a long period, it was commonly accepted that there is a finite and small number of "basic tastes" of which all seemingly complex tastes are ultimately composed. Just as with primary colors, the "basic" quality of those sensations derives chiefly from the nature of human perception, in this case the different sorts of tastes the human tongue can identify. Until the twenty-first century, most believed there were four basic tastes (bitterness, saltiness, sourness, and sweetness). More recently, a fifth taste, savory or umami, has been proposed by a large number of authorities associated with this field.[20] In Asian countries within the sphere of mainly Chinese, Indian and Japanese cultural influence, Piquance has traditionally been considered a sixth basic taste.

Bitterness

Bitterness is the most sensitive of the tastes, and many perceive it as unpleasant, sharp, or disagreeable. Common bitter foods and beverages include coffee, unsweetened cocoa, South American mate, marmalade, bitter melon, beer, bitters, olives, citrus peel, many plants in the Brassicaceae family, dandelion greens, wild chicory, escarole, and lemons. Quinine is also known for its bitter taste and is found in tonic water.

Bitterness is of interest to those who study evolution, as well as various health researchers[21][22] since a large number of natural bitter compounds are known to be toxic. The ability to detect bitter-tasting, toxic compounds at low thresholds is considered to provide an important protective function.[21][22][23] Plant leaves often contain toxic compounds, yet even amongst leaf-eating primates, there is a tendency to prefer immature leaves, which tend to be higher in protein and lower in fiber and poisons than mature leaves.[24] Amongst humans, various food processing techniques are used worldwide to detoxify otherwise inedible foods and make them palatable.[25]

The threshold for stimulation of bitter taste by quinine averages a concentration of 0.000008 M .[21] The taste thresholds of other bitter substances are rated relative to quinine, which is thus given a reference index of 1.[21][26] For example, Brucine has an index of 11, is thus perceived as intensely more bitter than quinine, and is detected at a much lower solution threshold.[21] The most bitter substance known is the synthetic chemical denatonium, which has an index of 1,000.[26] It is used as an aversive agent that is added to toxic substances to prevent accidental ingestion. This was discovered in 1958 during research on lignocaine, a local anesthetic, by Macfarlan Smith of Edinburgh, Scotland.

Research has shown that TAS2Rs (taste receptors, type 2, also known as T2Rs) such as TAS2R38 coupled to the G protein gustducin are responsible for the human ability to taste bitter substances.[27] They are identified not only by their ability to taste for certain "bitter" ligands, but also by the morphology of the receptor itself (surface bound, monomeric).[28] Recently it is speculated that the selective constraints on the TAS2R family have been weakened due to the relatively high rate of mutation and pseudogenization.[29] Researchers use two synthetic substances, phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) and 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) to study the genetics of bitter perception. These two substances taste bitter to some people, but are virtually tasteless to others. Among the tasters, some are so-called "supertasters" to whom PTC and PROP are extremely bitter. The variation in sensitivity is determined by two common alleles at the TAS2R38 locus.[30] This genetic variation in the ability to taste a substance has been a source of great interest to those who study genetics.

Saltiness

Saltiness is a taste produced primarily by the presence of sodium ions. Other ions of the alkali metals group also taste salty, but the further from sodium the less salty the sensation is. The size of lithium and potassium ions most closely resemble those of sodium and thus the saltiness is most similar. In contrast rubidium and cesium ions are far larger so their salty taste differs accordingly. The saltiness of substances is rated relative to sodium chloride (NaCl), which has an index of 1.[21][26] Potassium, as potassium chloride - KCl, is the principal ingredient in salt substitutes, and has a saltiness index of 0.6.[21][26]

Other monovalent cations, e.g. ammonium, NH4+, and divalent cations of the alkali earth metal group of the periodic table, e.g. calcium, Ca2+, ions generally elicit a bitter rather than a salty taste even though they, too, can pass directly through ion channels in the tongue, generating an action potential.

Sourness

Sourness is the taste that detects acidity. The sourness of substances is rated relative to dilute hydrochloric acid, which has a sourness index of 1. By comparison, tartaric acid has a sourness index of 0.7, citric acid an index of 0.46, and carbonic acid an index of 0.06.[21][26]

Sour taste is detected by a small subset of cells that are distributed across all taste buds in the tongue. Sour taste cells can be identified by expression of the protein PKD2L1,[31] although surprisingly this gene is not required for sour responses. The mechanism by which animals detect sour is still not completely understood. There is evidence that the protons that are abundant in sour substances can directly enter the sour taste cells. This transfer of positive charge into the cell can itself trigger an electrical response. It has also been proposed that weak acids such as acetic acid, which are not fully dissociated at physiological pH values, can penetrate taste cells and thereby elicit an electrical response. According to this mechanism, intracellular hydrogen ions inhibit potassium channels, which normally function to hyperpolarize the cell. By a combination of direct intake of hydrogen ions (which itself depolarizes the cell) and the inhibition of the hyperpolarizing channel, sourness causes the taste cell to fire action potentials and release neurotransmitter.

The most common food group that contains naturally sour foods is fruit, with examples such as lemon, grape, orange, and sometimes melon. Wine also usually has a sour tinge to its flavour. If not kept correctly, milk can spoil and contain a sour taste. Sour candy is especially popular in North America[32] including Cry Babies, Warheads, Lemon drops, Shock tarts and Sour Skittles and Starburst. Many of these candies contain citric acid.

The fear of sourness is called Acerophobia.

Sweetness

Sweetness, usually regarded as a pleasurable sensation, is produced by the presence of sugars and a few other substances. Sweetness is often connected to aldehydes and ketones, which contain a carbonyl group. Sweetness is detected by a variety of G protein coupled receptors coupled to the G protein gustducin found on the taste buds. At least two different variants of the "sweetness receptors" must be activated for the brain to register sweetness. Compounds the brain senses as sweet are thus compounds that can bind with varying bond strength to two different sweetness receptors. These receptors are T1R2+3 (heterodimer) and T1R3 (homodimer), which account for all sweet sensing in humans and animals.[33] Taste detection thresholds for sweet substances are rated relative to sucrose, which has an index of 1.[21][26] The average human detection threshold for sucrose is 10 millimoles per litre. For lactose it is 30 millimoles per litre, with a sweetness index of 0.3,[21] and 5-Nitro-2-propoxyaniline 0.002 millimoles per litre.

Umami

Umami is an appetitive taste[6] and is described as a savory[34][35] or meaty[35][36] taste. It can be tasted in cheese[37] and soy sauce,[19] and while also found in many other fermented and aged foods, this taste is also present in tomatoes, grains, and beans.[37] Monosodium glutamate (MSG), developed as a food additive in 1908 by Kikunae Ikeda,[5] produces a strong umami taste.[19] See TAS1R1 and TAS1R3 pages for a further explanation of the amino-acid taste receptor. A loanword from Japanese meaning "good flavour" or "good taste",[38] Umami (旨味?) is considered fundamental to many Eastern cuisines[39] and was first described in 1908,[40] although it was only recently recognized in the West as a basic taste.[19][41]

Some umami taste buds respond specifically to glutamate in the same way that "sweet" ones respond to sugar. Glutamate binds to a variant of G protein coupled glutamate receptors.[42][43]

Measuring relative tastes

Measuring the degree to which a substance presents one basic taste can be achieved in a subjective way by comparing its taste to a reference substance. Quinine, a bitter medicinal found in tonic water, can be used to subjectively rate the bitterness of a substance.[44] Units of dilute quinine hydrochloride (1 g in 2000 mL of water) can be used to measure the threshold bitterness concentration, the level at which the presence of a dilute bitter substance can be detected by a human taster, of other compounds.[44] More formal chemical analysis, while possible, is difficult.[44]

Relative saltiness can be rated by comparison to a dilute salt solution.[45]

The sourness of a substance can be rated by comparing it to very dilute hydrochloric acid (HCl).[46]

Sweetness is subjectively measured by comparing the threshold values, or level at which the presence of a dilute substance can be detected by a human taster, of different sweet substances.[47] Substances are usually measured relative to sucrose,[48] which is usually given an arbitrary index of 1[49][50] or 100.[51] Fructose is about 1.4 times sweeter than sucrose; glucose, a sugar found in honey and vegetables, is about three-quarters as sweet; and lactose, a milk sugar, is one-half as sweet.[b][47]

Functional structure

Bitterness

Research has shown that TAS2Rs (taste receptors, type 2, also known as T2Rs) such as TAS2R38 are responsible for the human ability to taste bitter substances.[52] They are identified not only by their ability to taste certain bitter ligands, but also by the morphology of the receptor itself (surface bound, monomeric).[53]

Saltiness

Saltiness is a taste produced best by the presence of cations (such as Na+, K+ or Li+)[54] and, like sour, it is tasted using ion channels.[54]

Other ions of the alkali metals group also taste salty, but the less sodium-like the ion is, the less salty the sensation. As the size of lithium and potassium ions is close to that of sodium, they taste similar to salt. In contrast, the larger rubidium and cesium ions do not taste as salty.

Other monovalent cations, e.g., ammonium, NH+
4
, and divalent cations of the alkali earth metal group of the periodic table, e.g., calcium, Ca2+, ions, in general, elicit a bitter rather than a salty taste even though they, too, can pass directly through ion channels in the tongue.

Sourness

Sourness is acidity,[55][56] and, like salt, it is a taste sensed using ion channels.[54] Hydrogen ion channels detect the concentration of hydronium ions that are formed from acids and water. In addition, the taste receptor PKD2L1 has been found to be involved in tasting sour.[57]

Sweetness

Sweetness is produced by the presence of sugars, some proteins, and a few other substances. It is often connected to aldehydes and ketones, which contain a carbonyl group. Sweetness is detected by a variety of G protein-coupled receptors coupled to a G protein that acts as an intermediary in the communication between taste bud and brain, gustducin.[58] These receptors are T1R2+3 (heterodimer) and T1R3 (homodimer), which account for sweet sensing in humans and other animals.[59]

Umami-ness

The amino acid glutamic acid is responsible for umami,[60][61] but some nucleotides (inosinic acid[39][62] and guanylic acid[60]) can act as complements, enhancing the taste.[39][62]

Glutamic acid binds to a variant of the G protein-coupled receptor, producing an umami taste.[63][64]

Further sensations

The tongue can also feel other sensations not generally included in the basic tastes. These are largely detected by the somatosensory system.

Calcium

In 2008, geneticists discovered a CaSR calcium receptor on the tongues of mice. The CaSR receptor is commonly found in the gastrointestinal tract, kidneys, and brain. Along with the "sweet" T1R3 receptor, the CaSR receptor can detect calcium as a taste. Whether closely related genes in mice and humans means the phenomenon exists in humans as well is unknown.[65][66]

Coolness

Some substances activate cold trigeminal receptors even when not at low temperatures. This "fresh" or "minty" sensation can be tasted in spearmint, menthol, ethanol, and camphor. Caused by activation of the same mechanism that signals cold, TRPM8 ion channels on nerve cells, unlike the actual change in temperature described for sugar substitutes, this coolness is only a perceived phenomenon.

Dryness

Some foods, such as unripe fruits, contain tannins or calcium oxalate that cause an astringent or rough sensation of the mucous membrane of the mouth. Examples include tea, red wine, rhubarb, and unripe persimmons and bananas.

Less exact terms for the astringent sensation are "dry", "rough", "harsh" (especially for wine), "tart" (normally referring to sourness), "rubbery", "hard" or "styptic".[67]

When referring to wine, dry is the opposite of sweet, and does not refer to astringency. Wines that contain tannins and so cause an astringent sensation are not necessarily classified as "dry," and "dry" wines are not necessarily astringent.

In the Indian Ayurvedic tradition, one of the six tastes is astringency (kasaaya).[68]

Fattiness

Recent research reveals a potential taste receptor called the CD36 receptor that reacts to fat (fatty acids, more specifically).[69] This receptor was found in mice.

Heartiness (kokumi)

Some Japanese researchers refer to the kokumi of foods laden with alcohol and thiol-groups in their amino acid extracts, and this sensation has also been described as mouthfeel.

Numbness

Both Chinese and Batak Toba cooking include the idea of 麻 ( or mati rasa), a tingling numbness caused by spices such as Sichuan pepper. The cuisines of Sichuan province in China and of the Indonesia province North Sumatra often combine this with chili pepper to produce a 麻辣 málà, "numbing-and-hot", or "mati rasa" flavour.[70] These sensations although not taste fall into a category of Chemesthesis.

Spiciness

Substances such as ethanol and capsaicin cause a burning sensation called Chemesthesis, piquance, spiciness, hotness, or prickliness by inducing a trigeminal nerve reaction together with normal taste reception. The sensation of heat is caused by the food's activating nerves that express TRPV1 and TRPA1 receptors. Two main plant-derived compounds that provide this sensation are capsaicin from chili peppers and piperine from black pepper. The piquant ("hot" or "spicy") sensation provided by chili peppers, black pepper, and other spices like ginger and horseradish plays an important role in a diverse range of cuisines across the world—especially in equatorial and sub-tropical climates, such as Ethiopian, Peruvian, Hungarian, Indian, Korean, Indonesian, Lao, Malaysian, Mexican, Southwest Chinese (including Szechuan cuisine), and Thai cuisines.

This particular sensation, called Chemesthesis, is not a taste in the technical sense, because the sensation does not arise from taste buds and a different set of nerve fibers carry it to the brain. Foods like chili peppers activate nerve fibers directly; the sensation interpreted as "hot" results from the stimulation of somatosensory (pain/temperature) fibers on the tongue. Many parts of the body with exposed membranes but no taste sensors (such as the nasal cavity, under the fingernails, surface of the eye ([cornea]) or a wound) produce a similar sensation of heat when exposed to hotness agents. Asian countries within the sphere of, mainly, Chinese, Indian, and Japanese cultural influence, traditionally consider piquance a sixth basic taste.

Temperature

Temperature can be an essential element of the taste experience. Food and drink that—in a given culture—is traditionally served hot is often considered distasteful if cold, and vice versa. For example, alcoholic beverages, with a few exceptions, are usually thought best when served cold, but soups—again, with exceptions—are usually only eaten hot. A cultural example is soda. In North America it is almost always preferred cold, regardless of season. In South America lukewarm soda is almost exclusively consumed in winter.

Other concepts

Supertasters

A supertaster is a person whose sense of taste is significantly more sensitive than average. The cause of this heightened response is likely, at least in part, due to an increased number of fungiform papillae.[71]

Aftertaste

Aftertastes arise after food has been swallowed. An aftertaste can differ from the food it follows. Medicines and tablets may also have a lingering aftertaste, as can certain artificial flavour compounds, such as aspartame (artificial sweetener).

Acquired taste

Acquired taste is an appreciation for a food or beverage that one is likely to initially dislike. Many of the world's delicacies are considered acquired tastes.

Innervation

Taste is brought to the brainstem by 3 different cranial nerves:

Disorders of taste

See also

Notes

Footnotes

a. ^ It has been known for some time that these categories may not be comprehensive. In Guyton's 1976 edition of Textbook of Medical Physiology, he wrote:

On the basis of physiologic studies, there are generally believed to be at least four primary sensations of taste: sour, salty, sweet, and bitter. Yet we know that a person can perceive literally hundreds of different tastes. These are all supposed to be combinations of the four primary sensations...However, there might be other less conspicuous classes or subclasses of primary sensations",[72]

b. ^ Some variation in values is not uncommon between various studies. Such variations may arise from a range of methodological variables, from sampling to analysis and interpretation. In fact there is a "plethora of methods"[73] Indeed, the taste index of 1, assigned to reference substances such as sucrose (for sweetness), hydrochloric acid (for sourness), quinine (for bitterness), and sodium chloride (for saltiness), is itself arbitrary for practical purposes.[46]

Some values, such as those for maltose and glucose, vary little. Others, such as aspartame and sodium saccharin, have much larger variation. Regardless of variation, the perceived intensity of substances relative to each reference substance remains consistent for taste ranking purposes. The indices table for McLaughlin & Margolskee (1994) for example,[21][74] is essentially the same as that of Svrivastava & Rastogi (2003),[75] Guyton & Hall (2006),[46] and Joesten et al. (2007).[49] The rankings are all the same, with any differences, where they exist, being in the values assigned from the studies from which they derive.

As for the assignment of 1 or 100 to the index substances, this makes no difference to the rankings themselves, only to whether the values are displayed as whole numbers or decimal points. Glucose remains about three-quarters as sweet as sucrose whether displayed as 75 or 0.75.

Citations

  1. ^ a b What Are Taste Buds? kidshealth.org
  2. ^ Human biology (Page 201/464) Daniel D. Chiras. Jones & Bartlett Learning, 2005.
  3. ^
  4. ^ Carlson, Neil R. (2010). "Psychology the Science of Behaviour". Toronto, Ontario: Pearson Canada Inc.. 
  5. ^ a b
  6. ^ a b Why do two great tastes sometimes not taste great together? scientificamerican.com. Dr. Tim Jacob, Cardiff University. May 22, 2009.
  7. ^ Miller, Greg (2). "Sweet here, salty there: Evidence of a taste map in the mammilian brain.". Science 333 (6047): 1213. doi:10.1126/science.333.6047.1213. 
  8. ^ Smell - The Nose Knows washington.edu, Eric H. Chudler.
  9. ^
  10. ^ Food texture: measurement and perception (page 4/311) Andrew J. Rosenthal. Springer, 1999.
  11. ^ On the Soul Aristotle. Translated by J. A. Smith. The Internet Classics Archive.
  12. ^ Aristotle's De anima (422b10-16) Ronald M. Polansky. Cambridge University Press, 2007.
  13. ^ Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function (Page 165/480) Stanley Finger. Oxford University Press US, 2001.
  14. ^ Ayurvedic balancing: an integration of Western fitness with Eastern wellness (Pages 25-26/188) Joyce Bueker. Llewellyn Worldwide, 2002.
  15. ^ The Six Tastes of Ayurveda ayurbalance.com, 2003
  16. ^ Bachmanov, A. A., and G. K. Beauchamp (2007), "Taste receptor genes", Annu Rev Nutr 27: 389–414, doi:10.1146/annurev.nutr.26.061505.111329, PMC 2721271, PMID 17444812, http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2721271. 
  17. ^ Chandrashekar J, Kuhn C, Oka Y, et al. (March 2010). "The cells and peripheral representation of sodium taste in mice". Nature 464 (7286): 297–301. doi:10.1038/nature08783. PMC 2849629. PMID 20107438. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2849629. 
  18. ^
  19. ^ a b c d "The Claim: The tongue is mapped into four areas of taste. Anahad O'connor.", The New York Times: Health section, November 10, 2008, http://www.nytimes.com/2008/11/11/health/11real.html?_r=1, retrieved 13 September 2010  May require free registration to view 
  20. ^ Ikeda, Kikunae (2002). "New Seasonings" (PDF). Chemical Senses 27 (9): 847–849. doi:10.1093/chemse/27.9.847. PMID 12438213. http://chemse.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/27/9/847. Retrieved 2007-12-30. . Acceptance of this as a "basic" taste came later, varying from region to region. see further: Savoriness
  21. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Guyton, Arthur C. (1991) Textbook of Medical Physiology. (8th ed). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders
  22. ^ a b Logue, A.W. (1986) The Psychology of Eating and Drinking. New York: W.H. Freeman & Co.
  23. ^ Glendinning, J. I. (1994). "Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive?". Physiol Behav 56 (6): 1217–1227. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(94)90369-7. PMID 7878094. 
  24. ^ Jones, S., Martin, R., & Pilbeam, D. (1994) The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  25. ^ Johns, T. (1990). With Bitter Herbs They Shall Eat It: Chemical ecology and the origins of human diet and medicine. Tucson: University of Arizona Press
  26. ^ a b c d e f McLaughlin S., Margolskee R.F. (1994). "The Sense of Taste". American Scientist 82 (6): 538–545. 
  27. ^ Maehashi, K., M. Matano, H. Wang, L. A. Vo, Y. Yamamoto, and L. Huang (2008). "Bitter peptides activate hTAS2Rs, the human bitter receptors". Biochem Biophys Res Commun 365 (4): 851–855. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2007.11.070. PMC 2692459. PMID 18037373. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2692459. 
  28. ^ Lindemann, Bernd (13 September 2001). "Receptors and transduction in taste" (PDF). Nature 413 (6852): 219–225. doi:10.1038/35093032. PMID 11557991. http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v413/n6852/pdf/413219a0.pdf. Retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  29. ^ Wang, X., S. D. Thomas, and J. Zhang (2004). "Relaxation of selective constraint and loss of function in the evolution of human bitter taste receptor genes". Hum Mol Genet 13 (21): 2671–2678. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddh289. PMID 15367488. 
  30. ^ Wooding, S., U. K. Kim, M. J. Bamshad, J. Larsen, L. B. Jorde, and D. Drayna (2004). "Natural selection and molecular evolution in PTC, a bitter-taste receptor gene". Am J Hum Genet 74 (4): 637–646. doi:10.1086/383092. PMC 1181941. PMID 14997422. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1181941. 
  31. ^ http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/08/060823184824.htm
  32. ^ http://www.hersheys.com/vending/lib/pdf/sellsheets/SweetSourSS.pdf
  33. ^ Zhao, Grace Q.; Yifeng Zhang, Mark A. Hoon, Jayaram Chandrashekar, Isolde Erlenbach, Nicholas J.P. Ryba, Charles S. Zuker (October 2003). "The Receptors for Mammalian Sweet and Savory taste" (PDF). Cell 115 (3): 255–266. doi:10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00844-4. PMID 14636554. http://download.cell.com/pdfs/0092-8674/PIIS0092867403008444.pdf. Retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  34. ^
  35. ^ a b "Merriam-Webster English Dictionary". Merriam-Webster, Incorporated. http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/umami. Retrieved 1 January 2011. 
  36. ^ "New Seasonings". http://chemse.oxfordjournals.org/content/27/9/847. 
  37. ^ a b What Is Umami?: Umami culture around the world Umami Information Center
  38. ^ 旨味 definition in English Denshi Jisho — Online Japanese dictionary
  39. ^ a b c Umami Food Ingredients Japan's Ministry of Agriculture, Forestry and Fisheries. 2007.
  40. ^ Yamaguchi, Shizuko & Ninomiya, Kumiko (1999), "Umami and Food Palatability", in Roy Teranishi, Emily L. Wick, & Irwin Hornstein (editors), Flavour Chemistry: Thirty Years of Progress, Proceedings of an American Chemical Society Symposium, held August 23–27, 1998, in Boston, Massachusetts, Published in New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 423–432, ISBN 0-306-46199-4, http://books.google.com/books?id=P3AggY-dWikC&pg=PA423&dq=umami&hl=en&ei=n6WNTPqEM4y4ceaanY0E&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=6&ved=0CEEQ6AEwBQ#v=onepage&q=umami&f=false, retrieved 13 September 2010 
  41. ^ "What exactly is umami?". The Umami Information Center. http://www.umamiinfo.com/what_exactly_is_umami?/. 
  42. ^ Lindemann, Bernd (February 2000). "A taste for Umami taste" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience 3 (2): 99–100. doi:10.1038/72153. PMID 10649560. http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v3/n2/pdf/nn0200_99.pdf. Retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  43. ^ Chaudhari, Nirupa; Ana Marie Landin, Stephen D. Roper (February 2000). "A metabotropic glutamate receptor variant functions as a taste receptor" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience 3 (2): 113–119. doi:10.1038/72053. PMID 10649565. http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v3/n2/pdf/nn0200_113.pdf. Retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  44. ^ a b c Quality control methods for medicinal plant materials, Pg. 38 World Health Organization, 1998.
  45. ^ Food Chemistry (Page 38/1070) H. D. Belitz, Werner Grosch, Peter Schieberle. Springer, 2009.
  46. ^ a b c Guyton, Arthur C; Hall, John; Hall, John E. (2006), Guyton and Hall Textbook of Medical Physiology (11th ed.), Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders, p. 664, ISBN 0-7216-0240-1  International ISBN 0-8089-2317-X 
  47. ^ a b Tsai, Michelle (14 May 2007), "How Sweet It Is? Measuring the intensity of sugar substitutes", Slate (The Washington Post Company), http://www.slate.com/id/2165999/, retrieved 14 September 2010 
  48. ^ Walters, D. Eric (Last updated 13 May 2008), "How is Sweetness Measured?", All About Sweeteners, http://www.sweetenerbook.com/measure.html, retrieved 15 September 2010 
  49. ^ a b Joesten, Melvin D; Hogg, John L; Castellion, Mary E (2007), "Sweeteness Relative to Sucrose (table)", The World of Chemistry: Essentials (4th ed.), Belmont, California: Thomson Brooks/Cole, p. 359, ISBN 0-495-01213-0, http://books.google.com/books?id=8hIoN3Q_zOkC&pg=PA359&lpg=PA359&dq=%22relative+to+sucrose%22&source=bl&ots=E1txi4DsSX&sig=wAbLIzj7Y5cCu2PeWOdiXfzr8nc&hl=en&ei=17ePTIK0LoXmvQPglYDqCw&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=6&ved=0CDAQ6AEwBTgK#v=onepage&q=%22relative%20to%20sucrose%22&f=false, retrieved 14 September 2010 
  50. ^ Coultate,Tom P (2009), "Sweetness relative to sucrose as an arbitrary standard", Food: The Chemistry of its Components (5th ed.), Cambridge, UK: Royal Society of Chemistry, pp. 268–269, ISBN 978-0-85404-111-4, http://books.google.com/books?id=KF2A8Cz7B-cC&pg=PA268&lpg=PA268&dq=%22relative+to+sucrose%22&source=bl&ots=fgH81scq2_&sig=Sx6J_yj9oD2n3zbFadwwGRi5sIY&hl=en&ei=kfSPTLfdMM6DcMLT_L8M&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=4&ved=0CCAQ6AEwAzgo#v=onepage&q=%22relative%20to%20sucrose%22&f=false, retrieved 15 September 2010 
  51. ^ Mehta, Bhupinder & Mehta, Manju (2005), "Sweetness of sugars", Organic Chemistry, India: Prentice-Hall, p. 956, ISBN 8120-32441-2, http://books.google.com/books?id=QV6cwXA9XkEC&pg=PA956&dq=%22Organic+Chemistry+%22+taste&hl=en&ei=M7GQTL_NOIT-vQOv67HfCw&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=4&ved=0CEAQ6AEwAw#v=onepage&q&f=false, retrieved 15 September 2010  Alternative ISBN 978-8120-32441-1 
  52. ^ Maehashi, K., M. Matano, H. Wang, L. A. Vo, Y. Yamamoto, and L. Huang (2008), "Bitter peptides activate hTAS2Rs, the human bitter receptors", Biochem Biophys Res Commun 365 (4): 851–855, doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2007.11.070, PMC 2692459, PMID 18037373, http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2692459. 
  53. ^ Lindemann, Bernd (13 September 2001), "Receptors and transduction in taste" (PDF), Nature 413 (6852): 219–225, doi:10.1038/35093032, PMID 11557991, http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v413/n6852/pdf/413219a0.pdf, retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  54. ^ a b c Transduction channels in sensory cells (Page 155/304) Stephan Frings, Jonathan Bradley. Wiley-VCH, 2004.
  55. ^ outlines of chemistry with practical work (Page 241) Henry John Horstman Fenton. CUP Archive.
  56. ^ Focus Ace Pmr 2009 Science (Page 242/522) Chang See Leong, Chong Kum Ying,Choo Yan Tong & Low Swee Neo. Focus Ace Pmr 2009 Science.
  57. ^ "Biologists Discover How We Detect Sour Taste", Science Daily, August 24, 2006, http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/08/060823184824.htm, retrieved 12 September 2010 
  58. ^ How the Taste Bud Translates Between Tongue and Brain nytimes.com, August 4, 1992.
  59. ^ Zhao, Grace Q.; Yifeng Zhang, Mark A. Hoon, Jayaram Chandrashekar, Isolde Erlenbach, Nicholas J.P. Ryba, Charles S. Zuker (October 2003), "The Receptors for Mammalian Sweet and Savory taste" (PDF), Cell 115 (3): 255–266, doi:10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00844-4, PMID 14636554, http://download.cell.com/pdfs/0092-8674/PIIS0092867403008444.pdf, retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  60. ^ a b What Is Umami?: What Exactly is Umami? Umami Information Center
  61. ^ Chandrashekar, Jayaram; Hoon, Mark A; Ryba , Nicholas J. P. & Zuker, Charles S (16 November 2006), "The receptors and cells for mammalian taste", Nature 444 (7117): 288–294, doi:10.1038/nature05401, PMID 17108952, https://wiki.brown.edu/confluence/download/attachments/1444406/nature05401.pdf, retrieved 13 September 2010 
  62. ^ a b What Is Umami?: The Composition of Umami Umami Information Center
  63. ^ Lindemann, Bernd (February 2000), "A taste for Umami taste" (PDF), Nature Neuroscience 3 (2): 99–100, doi:10.1038/72153, PMID 10649560, http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v3/n2/pdf/nn0200_99.pdf, retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  64. ^ Chaudhari, Nirupa; Ana Marie Landin, Stephen D. Roper (February 2000), "A metabotropic glutamate receptor variant functions as a taste receptor" (PDF), Nature Neuroscience 3 (2): 113–119, doi:10.1038/72053, PMID 10649565, http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v3/n2/pdf/nn0200_113.pdf, retrieved 2007-12-30. 
  65. ^ Tordorf, Michael G. (2008), "Chemosensation of Calcium", American Chemical Society National Meeting, Fall 2008, 236th, Philadelphia, PA: American Chemical Society, AGFD 207, http://portal.acs.org/portal/acs/corg/content?_nfpb=true&_pageLabel=PP_TRANSITIONMAIN&node_id=859&use_sec=false&sec_url_var=region1 
  66. ^ "That Tastes ... Sweet? Sour? No, It's Definitely Calcium!", Science Daily, August 21, 2008, http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2008/08/080820163008.htm, retrieved 14 September 2010 
  67. ^ http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/68000103/abstract
  68. ^ http://www.ayurshop.com/diet/rasas.html
  69. ^ Potential Taste Receptor for Fat Identified: Scientific American
  70. ^ Spice Pages: Sichuan Pepper (Zanthoxylum, Szechwan peppercorn, fagara, hua jiao, sansho 山椒, timur, andaliman, tirphal)
  71. ^ Bartoshuk L. M., Duffy V. B. et al. (1994). "PTC/PROP tasting: anatomy, psychophysics, and sex effects." 1994". Physiol Behav 56 (6): 1165–71. 
  72. ^ Guyton, Arthur C. (1976), Textbook of Medical Physiology (5th ed.), Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders, p. 839, ISBN 0-7216-4393-0 
  73. ^ Macbeth, Helen M. & MacClancy, Jeremy, ed. (2004), "plethora of methods characterising human taste perception", Researching Food Habits: Methods and Problems, The anthropology of food and nutrition, Vol. 5, New York: Berghahn Books, pp. 87–88, ISBN 1-57181-544-9, http://books.google.com/books?id=ZLQTSqfB5igC&pg=PA88&dq=plethora+methods+bartoshuk&hl=en&ei=8TGQTI_lJ8jCcc3juc8M&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=1&ved=0CCoQ6AEwAA#v=onepage&q=plethora%20methods%20bartoshuk&f=false, retrieved 15 September 2010=  Paperback ISBN 1-57181-545-7 
  74. ^ McLaughlin, Susan, & Margolskee, Rorbert F (November–December 1994), The Sense of Taste American Scientist, 82, pp. 538–545 
  75. ^ Svrivastava, R.C. & Rastogi, R.P (2003), "Relative taste indices of some substances", in ., Transport Mediated by Electrical Interfaces, Studies in interface science, vol.18, Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Science, ISBN 0-444-51453-8 B.V, http://books.google.com/books?id=hIyM_o4YFZ4C&pg=PA274&dq=%22same+concentration+(1M)%22&hl=en&ei=6yqRTIz4Ho-evQOM4dnBCw&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=1&ved=0CCwQ6AEwAA#v=onepage&q=%22same%20concentration%20(1M)%22&f=false, retrieved 12 September 2010  Taste indices of table 9, p.274 are select sample taken from table in Guyton's Textbook of Medical Physiology (present in all editions) 

Further reading

External links