Halkomelem language

Halkomelem
Spoken in Canada, United States
Region Southwestern British Columbia into Northern Washington
Ethnicity Sto:lo people
Native speakers 225 (200 in Canada; 25 in United States)  (date missing)
Language family
Salishan
  • Central Salish
    • Halkomelem
Language codes
ISO 639-3 hur

Halkomelem (also Halq'eméylem in the Upriver dialect, Hul'q'umín'um' in the Island dialect, and Hun'qumi'num' in the Downriver dialect)[1] is a language of the First Nations peoples of southeastern Vancouver Island from the west shore of Saanich Inlet northward beyond Nanoose Bay, and of the mainland around the Fraser River Delta upriver to Harrison Lake and the lower boundary of Fraser Canyon in British Columbia. In the classification of Salishan languages, Halkomelem is a member of the Central Salish branch. There are four other branches of the family: Tsamosan, Interior Salish, Bella Coola, and Tillamook. Speakers of the Central and Tsamosan languages are often identified in ethnographic literature as "Coast Salish."

The word "Halkomelem" is an anglicization for the language which has three distinguishable dialect groups: (1) an Island group, spoken by six separate but closely related First Nations in the Georgia Strait area around the mouth of the Fraser River: the Chemainus, Cowichan, Halalt, Lake Cowichan, Lyackson, and Penelakut,[2] (2) a Downriver group, the Lower Sto:lo,spoken by the Tsawwassen, Musqueam, Tsleil-Waututh, Qayqayt, Kwikwetlem, and Katzie First Nations, and (3) an Upriver group, the Upper Sto:lo, spoken from the Matsqui First Nation on upstream. The language differences (namely, in phonology and lexicon) are greatest between the Island and Upriver dialects, with the Downriver dialect (especially the Tsawwassen First Nation) providing a central link between the other two. The diversity of the Halkomelem dialects is noted to be the result of complex social and economic forces and linguistic change, as many Island people crossed the Georgia Strait to camp along the Fraser River (in both the Downriver and Upriver areas) for the summer runs of salmon. Arranged marriages between children in different dialect areas was also common, helping to establish a regional social network in the Strait of Georgia-Puget Sound Basin.

The Halkomelem language is near extinction largely due to the extensive transportation of First Nations children to residential schools. In 2000, it was estimated that the number of fluent Halkomelem speakers was less than one dozen. Most are middle-aged or older, and few are monolingual, as there was a flood of English-speaking settlers in the region in the mid-nineteenth century. Language programs at the Stó:lō Nation, Seabird Island First Nation, and Cowichan First Nation have been developed to save the language. A program aimed at adults at Musqueam is a collaboration between the band and the University of British Columbia First Nations Language Program.

Contents

Phonology[3]

Note: All examples are drawn from Musqueam, a band which speaks the Downriver dialect of Halkomelem. Relevant differences in the phonology of the Island and Upriver dialects are noted at the foot of the phoneme charts.

Vowels

Musqueam has five vowel phonemes. Long and short vowels (but not schwa) contrast. Vowel length is written in the native orthography as ⟨·⟩.

   Short   Long 
 Front   Central   Back (rounded)   Front   Central   Back (rounded) 
 High  i u ⟨i·⟩ ⟨u·⟩
 Mid  e ə ⟨e·⟩
 Low  a ⟨a·⟩

Four of these vowel phonemes (/i/, /e/, /a/, and /ə/) are common, while /u/ occurs mainly in loan words.

All five vowel phonemes vary considerably phonetically. The phoneme /i/ has three distinct allophones. It is realized as [e] following unrounded uvulars. It is realized as [ɪ] with a central off-glide preceding both unrounded and rounded uvulars. Elsewhere, it is realized as low [i] or high [e]. The /e/ is realized as a low to mid-front vowel, usually between [ɛ] or high [æ]. The /a/ is low and central to back, often close to [ɑ]. The /u/ is high, back, and rounded, realized somewhere between low [u] or high [o].

When stressed, the schwa /ə/ appears in most environments as a mid-central, but it is fronted and raised before /x/, approaching [ɪ]; before /y/ it is also fronted, approaching [ɛ]; before /w/ it is lower and back, approaching [ɑ]; and before rounded velars it is mid-back, close to [o]. Unstressed /ə/ can be as high as [i] before /x/ and /y/, and before labialized velars it is realized as [o] or [u]. This phoneme can also be assimilated to a stressed /e/ or a stressed /a/ in an adjacent syllable, by vowel harmony.

Consonants

Labial Dental Alveolar Alv. sibilant Lateral Palatal Velar Labialized Velar Uvular Labialized Uvular Glottal
Plosive plain p [p] (tθ) [tθ] t [t] c [ts] (č) [t͡ʃ] (k) [k] [kʷ] q [q] [qʷ]
glottalized [pʼ] tʼθ [tθʼ] [tʼ] [tsʼ] ƛʼ [tɬʼ] (kʼ) [kʼ] kʼʷ [kʷʼ] [qʼ] qʼʷ[qʷʼ] ʔ [ʔ]
voiced {b} [b] {d} [d] {j}[d͡ʒ]
Fricative voiceless {f} [f] θ [θ] s [s] ɬ [t͡ʃ] (š) [ʃ] x [x] [xʷ] [χ] x̌ʷ [χʷ] h [h]
Resonant plain m [m] n [n] l [l] y [j] w [w] {r} [ʀ]
glottalized (mʼ) [mˀ] (nʼ) [nˀ] (lʼ) [lˀ] (yʼ) [jˀ] (wʼ) [wˀ]

Of the forty-three consonants that have been recorded by Suttles (2004) in the Musqueam Downriver dialect of Halkomelem, five (enclosed in parentheses) are of recent and/or peripheral phonemic status, five glottalized resonants (enclosed in square brackets) pose a problem in phonemic analysis (although they occur frequently), and five (enclosed in braces) occur only in a few foreign and imitative words. The stops and affricates are grouped together for simplification purposes.

The plain plosives are less aspirate before vowels than in English, but they are more aspirate finally. Although the glottalized plosives are ejectives, they are not usually strongly released.

Suttles (2004) made several interesting notes on the Musqueam obstruents. The labiodental fricative /f/ occurs in recent loans from English and their derivatives such as in káfi “coffee” and in číf “chief.” According to Suttles (2004), the stops /t/ and /tʼ/ are articulated at a point slightly forward of that of the usual English “t” and “d,” while the affricates /c/ and /cʼ/ are somewhat more retracted than these English phonemes. The affricate [j] has only been recorded in kinjáj “Englishmen” and kinjájqən “English (language).” The glottalized lateral affricate /ƛʼ/ [tʼɬ] is produced when the apex of the tongue at the onset is in the position for the lateral release rather than for a /t/, and there is less friction produced than with other affricates. The phonemes /k/ and /kʼ/ occur in “baby talk” as substitutes for /q/ and /qʼ/. The uvular fricative /x̌/ [χ] is produced with a great deal of friction and/or uvular vibration, and it contrasts strongly with the velar fricative /x/.

There is variation in the extent to which Musqueam speakers glottalize resonants. Phonetically, there are glottalized resonants and resonants preceded or followed by glottal stops, however, Suttles (2004) found no instances of contrastive distribution among any of the three. He puts forth two explanations for these facts: that there are two sequences of phonemes, /Rʔ/ and /ʔR/, with overlapping [Rˀ] allophones, or that there is a single phoneme /Rˀ/ that is realized in three distinct ways. In preferring the latter explanation, Suttles holds that there may be five glottalized resonant phonemes in the dialect, although Downriver speakers glottalize resonants very lightly, making them difficult to detect.

In most Upriver dialects, glottalized resonants do not exist, while in Island dialects, they are more sharply articulated (tenseness is a key feature of Island speech). As is the case with many other phonological features, Downriver Halkomelem stands as a link between the other dialect areas, and it is possible that its speakers vary depending on Island or Upriver influence. Other differences between dialects include: Island and Downriver have both /n/ and /l/, while Upriver has merged these as /l/. Upriver lacks the post-vocalic glottal stops of the other two dialects, and vowel length usually appears in that environment for speakers of that dialect. Additionally, Upriver dialects have greater pitch differences, and some words are differentiated by pitch alone.

Stress and Pitch

Based on Suttles’ (2004) recordings of several speakers of the Downriver (Musqueam) dialect, stress in Halkomelem consists of an increase in intensity and an accompanying rise in pitch. The three levels of stress are primary (marked /׳/), secondary (marked /`/), and weak (unmarked). There is one vowel with primary stress in every full word, however, its occurrence is not completely predictable.

In uninflected words with more than one vowel, the primary stress usually falls on the first vowel (e.g. as in céləx “hand” and léləmʼ “house”). There are exceptions to this general pattern (e.g. as in xəmén “enemy”). As shown by the preceding example, if the word contains both a full vowel and one or more schwas, the stress is placed on the full vowel. Again, there are exceptions to this pattern, such as in words with a final glottal stop that cannot be preceded by schwa (e.g. as in nə́cʼaʔ “one”).

Although minimal pairs contrasting stress are rare, they do exist in the language. The primary stress of a verb root consisting of a resonant, a schwa, and an obstruent followed by the suffix /-t/ “transitive” can fall on either the root or the suffix, allowing for minimal pairs such as mə̀kʼʷət “salvage it” and məkʼʷə́t “finish it all.”

The secondary stress appears most often in words that are composed of a root that has retained its stress and a stressed suffix (e.g. as in cʼéwəθàmx “help me”). It may be the case, however, that the secondary stress recorded by Suttles (2004) in words like cʼéwəθàmx is actually a falling pitch; this seems to be characteristic of the last stressed syllable of a phrase in the language. Additional analyses of the sentential intonation patterns are needed.

Phonotactics

All obstruents (except the glottals) typically follow one another in sequences of up to four, although a sequence of five is also possible (e.g. as in txʷstx̌ʷásʔal “just standing in shock”). There are no specific restrictions on the types of obstruent sequences that can occur. Plosives appearing in sequences are rearticulated, and sequences of /ss/ are common in the language.

Resonants only appear adjacent to vowels. When these sounds occur in the middle of words, they are found in sequences of resonant-obstruent, resonant-resonant, and obstruent-resonant. An initial resonant is always followed a vowel, and a final resonant must be preceded by one.

The laryngeals are more restricted than members of the other natural classes in Halkomelem. The glottal stop occurs only adjacent to a vowel, and, within words, it does not follow any obstruent except (the prefix) /s/. It can never occur in final position following a schwa. /h/ occurs only before vowels, following a resonant or one of the fricatives at morpheme boundaries, but never following other obstruents. Interestingly, it can appear between an unstressed and a stressed vowel, but it cannot occur between a stressed and an unstressed vowel.

Morphophonemics

Certain processes affect the realization of underlying sounds in Halkomelem. Alternations that occur fairly commonly are discussed in this section, rather than in the following section on morphology.

Morphology[4]

Like the majority of Salishan languages, Halkomelem is polysynthetic. A word in Halkomelem may consist of a root standing alone and unaltered, or of a root altered by one or more processes of internal modification and/or accompanied by one or more affixes. Since all words (with the exception of a few adverbs) can function as predicate heads, there is no basis for distinguishing verbs, nouns, and adjectives. There are other bases, however, for distinguishing these classes. Verbs have progressive forms and do not take possessive affixes, while nouns do not have progressive forms and do take possessive affixes. Adjectives have neither progressive forms, nor do they take possessive affixes. Compounding is non-existent in the language, although some scholars believe to have found a few possible examples.

The majority of verb roots have the shapes CAC, CəC, CəCC, while noun roots typically have the shape CVCVC (V is any vowel). The most common shapes of adjective roots are CəC and CAC. There is a prefix that nominalizes verbs and adjectives, and there are several prefixes that make verbs out of nouns. Additionally, there are several ways to make adjective-like words from nouns. Processes of internal modification of the root include reduplication (of initial CV and CVC), shift in stress and vowel grade, and glottalization of resonants (which also affects suffixes). Roots of different shapes often undergo different processes to produce forms that are grammatically identical.

Verbs

Verbs roots are identified as perfective, as opposed to progressive, aspect.

Several verbs also have a durative aspect, which can occur in both forms.

A number also have an iterative-dispositional aspect. For a few of these verb roots, this aspect can appear in both a progressive and in a perfective form.

The majority of verbs have a resultative form which is adjective-like and does not carry a progressive-perfective distinction.

The plural can be optionally marked in all of these forms. The diminutive is also marked, optionally, in only the progressive and resultative aspects.

Nouns

It is possible to internally modify noun roots in Halkomelem for the plural, the diminutive, and the diminutive plural. Compare:

A few nouns may have resultative forms. They do not have progressive forms, but they may be made into a verb with a verbalizing affix and then express this form.

Adjectives

Similar to noun roots, adjective roots can be internally modified for plural, diminutive, and diminutive plural. They can only have progressive forms if made into verbs by means of a verbalizing affix.

Complex adjectives are formed from adjective roots and lexical suffixes.

Affixes

Halkomelem contains prefixes, suffixes, and infixes. All infixes of the language have been described in the preceding sections. Affixes are typically divisible into inflectional or derivational and grammatical or lexical categories, depending on their involvement in paradigms and meaning, however, a number of Halkomelem affixes mix these categories. Suttles (2004) identifies the following classes of suffixes and prefixes; a sampling of these affixes follow.

Most verbs roots are semantically inactive/patient oriented (e.g. they have glosses like “get hit” or “get washed”), while few verbs are semantically active/agent oriented (e.g. “look” or “see”). All are grammatically intransitive and take subjects only. These relations are different with the suffixes of the voice system. A verb that is made up of an inactive root and an intransitive suffix is grammatically intransitive, but semantically active. An inactive or active root that takes on a transitive suffix is grammatically transitive and takes an object. The transitive suffix is the base for an object or passive person suffix. Two of the most commonly used transitive suffixes distinguish actions performed with limited control or accidentally from those performed with full control or purposely.

Aspectual prefixes, which precede predicate heads, have adverbial meaning and express temporal distinctions. Modal suffixes follow the suffixes of the voice system and indicate desire or intention and search or arrangement.

Lexical suffixes can be related to verb roots as objects, locus, or instruments; to adjective roots as noun heads; and to noun roots as noun possessors or the noun heads of modifiers.

The personal affixes distinguish first, second, and third person in singular and plural. There are neither dual forms nor inclusive/exclusive distinctions in this language. There is also a set of possessive affixes (prefixes for first and second person singular, suffixes for first-person plural and third person, and a combination of prefix and suffix for second-person plural). This system will be covered, in detail, in the "Syntax" section.

The Ordering of Affixes

Derivational prefixes and suffixes form an inner layer around the word root, while inflectional affixes form an outer layer around the root. Among derivational affixes, those with lexical meaning stand closer to the root than those with purely grammatical meaning. Among inflectional affixes, those of the voice and person systems stand closer to the root than the aspectual prefixes and modal suffixes.

Syntax[5]

In Musqueam, a sentence minimally consists of a predicate. Predicate heads can be bare roots (e.g. cákʼʷ "far"), derived forms (e.g. spéʔeθ "black bar"), inflected forms (e.g. cʼéwət "help him/her/them"), and forms including both derivational and inflectional affixes (e.g. kʷə́xnəct "name-base-transitive," as in "name a price"). Predicate heads can also be words that are definable morphologically. This includes verbs (e.g. ném "go"), adjectives (e.g. θí "big"), nouns (e.g. swə́yʼqeʔ "man"), members of the closed sets of personal words (see the following section), and interrogative words (e.g. stém "what").

A verbal predicate may be expanded with the addition of one or two auxiliary verbs before the head and/or one or more adverbs preceding or following the head.

Verb heads are also found with verb complements and compound verbs.

Like verbal predicates, adjectival and nominal predicates can be expanded with other elements. Nominal predicate heads can appear alone, followed by particles and adverbs.

Syntactically, adjectival and nominal predicate heads appear with auxiliaries less often than verbs. Adjectives and nouns can appear together in nominal predicates expanded with adjectives. A nominal predicate head can be preceded by a modifying adjective or numeral.

Adjectives usually appear as predicate heads accompanied by particles only, but they can be preceded by auxiliaries and adverbs acting as intensifiers.

Person Markers[6]

The Halkomelem person markers (forms that correspond in meaning to English personal pronouns) include a set of affixes, one set of particles, and two sets of words (personal and possessive). As mentioned in the "Morphology" section, there is no dual number or inclusive/exclusive distinction in the language, however, some scholars believe that the forms identified here as second-person "singular" were once used in addressing a married couple, a pair of brothers, or even a family, while the "plural" forms were used for a larger or less integrated group.

First- and Second-Person Subject Particles

The first- and second-person subject particles pattern like a nominative–accusative case marking system. In other words, the same particles mark first- and second-person subjects in both intransitive and transitive predicates in main clauses (coordinate constructions).

Singular Plural
First-Person cən ct
Second-Person čxʷ ce·p

Suttles (2004) classified the first- and second-person subject particles as second-position predicate particles, along with about twenty other particles that can appear within the predicate. Most of the second-position predicate particles are mobile, appearing after the first word of the predicate (whatever that may be). If the only word in the predicate is the head, the first- and second-person subject particles will follow it; if the head is preceded by an auxiliary verb, they will follow the auxiliary; if the head is preceded by an adverb, they will follow the adverb.

Object Person Markers

An object person marker can only be suffixed to a transitive verb (i.e. a verb stem that has a transitive suffix). The four forms appear to be composed of identifiable elements: /-S/ and /-am/ "non-third-person singular," /-al-/ "non-third-person plural," /-x/ and /-xʷ/ "first person," and /-ə/ second person. Third-person objects are unmarked.

Singular Plural
First-Person -Samx ~ -amx -alʼxʷ
Second-Person -Samə ~ -amə -alə

The element /-S/ occurs with the transitivizer /-t/, and they coalesce as /θ/. With the root cʼéw- "help" and /-t/ "transitive," we find:

These forms are normally accompanied by subject person markers.

Third-Person Subjects

The third-person subjects comprise an ergative–absolutive system. With a transitive predicate head, a third-person subject, like a third-person object, is marked by zero. Plurality is optionally indicated by the particle ʔé·ɬtən.

With a transitive predicate head in a main clause, on the other hand, a third-person subject must be marked by the suffix /-əs/. It always follows the transitivizer and object person marker, if any. Unlike the first- and second-person subject particles, the suffix does not move to follow an auxiliary or adverb. Again, the plurality of the third-person may be indicated by the particle ʔé·ɬtən.

Constraints

In the active paradigm, a third person cannot be the subject with a second person as the object. Instead, we find passive forms.

Only third persons can be agents in the passive. Other relations (e.g. the forms "*I am seen by you" or "*he is seen by me") can be expressed only in the active (e.g. "you see me" and "I see him").

Subordinate Clauses

A subordinate clause is produced by prefixing on of the two subordinating particles, /wə-/ "if, when, that" and /ʔəl/ "whenever, whatever," to the first word in a predicate and replacing its coordinate subject person marker with a subordinate subject person marker. Subordinate subject person markers are the same in both intransitive and transitive active predicates.

Singular Plural
First-Person -e·n ~ -ən -ət
Second-Person -əxʷ -e·p ~ -əp
Third-Person -əs same as singular

Subordinate clauses usually follow main clauses, but there are a few exceptions.

Basic Halq'eméylem words and phrases

English Halq'eméylem [7]
Hello/Greetings Kwéleches
How are you? Lichewx we eyo
I am fine Tsel we eyo
Thank you Kw'as hoy
What is your name? Tewat te' skwix
1 Letse
2 Isa:le
3 Lhi:xw
4 Xe'o:thels
5 Lheq'a:tses
6 T'xem
7 Tho:kws
8 Teqa:tsa
9 Tu:xw
10 O:pel

Peoples speaking Halkomelem

Hul'qumi'num (Hul’q’umi’num’: Island dialect)

Hunquminum (Hǝn̓q̓ǝmin̓ǝm̓: Downriver dialect)

Halq'emeylem (Halq’eméylem: Upriver dialect)

See also

External links

Notes

  1. ^ http://www.ydli.org/langs/halkomelem.htm
  2. ^ Hul'quim'num Treaty Group
  3. ^ Suttles, Wayne. (2004). Musqueam Reference Grammar. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, 3-22.
  4. ^ Suttles, Wayne. (2004). Musqueam Reference Grammar. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, 23-30.
  5. ^ Suttles, Wayne. (2004). Musqueam Reference Grammar. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, 31-72.
  6. ^ Suttles, Wayne. (2004). Musqueam Reference Grammar. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, 320-329.
  7. ^ Halq'eméylem Language by Helen Carr, Kwantlen First Nation

References