Capgras delusion

Capgras delusion
Classification and external resources
DiseasesDB 32606

The Capgras delusion theory (or Capgras syndrome) is a disorder in which a person holds a delusion that a friend, spouse, parent, or other close family member has been replaced by an identical-looking impostor. The Capgras delusion is classified as a delusional misidentification syndrome, a class of delusional beliefs that involves the misidentification of people, places, or objects. It can occur in acute, transient, or chronic forms. Cases in which patients hold the belief that time has been "warped" or "substituted" have also been reported.[1]

The delusion is most common in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia, although it can occur in connection with a number of conditions, including brain injury and dementia.[2] It has also been reported as occuring in association with diabetes, hypothyroidism and migraine attacks.[3] It occurs more frequently in females, with a female:male ratio of 3:2.[4] Although the Capgras delusion is commonly called a syndrome, because it can occur as part of, or alongside, various other disorders and conditions, some researchers have argued that it should be considered a symptom, rather than a syndrome or classification in its own right.

Contents

History

The Capgras delusion is named after Joseph Capgras (1873–1950), a French psychiatrist who first described the disorder in 1923 in his paper co-authored by Reboul-Lachaux,[5] on the case of a French woman who complained that corresponding "doubles" had taken the places of her husband and other people she knew.

Their term l'illusion des « sosies »...—which can be literally translated as "the illusion of 'doubles'..."—finds some[6] modern professional use in French (e.g. "L’illusion des sosies de Capgras est...", which however its authors render in English as "Capgras’[7] syndrome is ...").[8] But the distinction between illusion and delusion is important in modern technical English, and the Capgras syndrome is clearly a delusional condition.

Presentation

This case is taken from a 1991 report by Passer and Warnock:[9]

Mrs. D, a 74-year-old married housewife, recently discharged from a local hospital after her first psychiatric admission, presented to our facility for a second opinion. At the time of her admission earlier in the year, she had received the diagnosis of atypical psychosis because of her belief that her husband had been replaced by another unrelated man. She refused to sleep with the impostor, locked her bedroom and door at night, asked her son for a gun, and finally fought with the police when attempts were made to hospitalise her. At times she believed her husband was her long deceased father. She easily recognised other family members and would misidentify her husband only.

Causes

Some of the first clues to the possible causes of the Capgras delusion were suggested by the study of brain-injured patients who had developed prosopagnosia. In this condition, patients are unable to recognize faces consciously, despite being able to recognize other types of visual objects. However, a 1984 study by Bauer showed that even though conscious face recognition was impaired, patients with the condition showed autonomic arousal (measured by a galvanic skin response measure) to familiar faces,[10] suggesting that there are two pathways to face recognition—one conscious and one unconscious.

In a 1990 paper published in the British Journal of Psychiatry, psychologists Hadyn Ellis and Andy Young hypothesized that patients with Capgras delusion may have a "mirror image" of prosopagnosia, in that their conscious ability to recognize faces was intact, but they might have damage to the system that produces the automatic emotional arousal to familiar faces.[11] This might lead to the experience of recognizing someone while feeling something was not "quite right" about them.

In 1997, Hadyn Ellis and his colleagues published a study of five patients with Capgras delusion (all diagnosed with schizophrenia) and confirmed that although they could consciously recognize the faces, they did not show the normal automatic emotional arousal response.[12]

In 1997, William Hirstein and Vilayanur S. Ramachandran reported similar findings in a paper published on a single case of a patient with Capgras delusion after brain injury.[13] Ramachandran also portrays this case in his book Phantoms in the Brain.[14] Ramachandran also gave a talk about it at TED 2007 video can be found here Since the patient was capable of feeling emotions and recognizing faces but could not feel emotions when recognizing familiar faces, Ramachandran hypothesizes that the origin of Capgras syndrome is a disconnection between the temporal cortex, where faces are usually recognized (see temporal lobe), and the limbic system, involved in emotions. Because the patient could not put together memories and feelings, he believed objects in a photograph were new on every viewing, even though they normally should have evoked feelings (e.g., a person close to him, a familiar object, or even himself). Ramachandran therefore believed there was a relationship between Capgras syndrome and a more general difficulty in linking successive episodic memories, since it is believed that emotion is critical for creating memories.

Most likely, more than an impairment of the automatic emotional arousal response is necessary to form Capgras delusion, as the same pattern has been reported in patients showing no signs of delusions.[15] Ellis and colleagues suggested that a second factor explains why this unusual experience is transformed into a delusional belief; this second factor is thought to be an impairment in reasoning, although no definitive impairment has been found to explain all cases.[16]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Aziza VM, Warnerb NJ (2005). "Capgras' Syndrome of Time". Psychopathology 38 (1): 49–52. doi:10.1159/000083970. PMID 15722649. 
  2. ^ Förstl H, Almeida OP, Owen AM, Burns A, Howard R (November 1991). "Psychiatric, neurological and medical aspects of misidentification syndromes: a review of 260 cases". Psychol Med 21 (4): 905–10. doi:10.1017/S0033291700029895. PMID 1780403. 
  3. ^ Bhatia,M.S (1990). "Capgras syndrome in a patient with migraine". BJP 157 (6): 917–18. doi:10.1192/bjp.157.6.917. 
  4. ^ Giannini AJ, Black HR. The Psychiatric, Psychogenic and Somatopsychic Disorders Handbook. Garden City, NY: Medical Examination. pp. 97–8. ISBN 0-87488-596-5. 
  5. ^ Capgras J, Reboul-Lachaux J (1923). "Illusion des " sosies " dans un délire systématisé chronique". Bulletin de la Société Clinique de Médicine Mentale 2: 6–16. http://www.bium.univ-paris5.fr/histmed/medica/cote?epo1250. 
  6. ^ "Approche clinique du syndrome de Capgras ou « illusion des sosies » illustrée par un cas", Gaël Le Vacon, 2006; rough translation: "Clinical approach to Capgras sydrome or 'illusion of doubles' illustrated by a case"
  7. ^ Sic on linked Web page, though Dorland's Illustrated Medical Dictionary, 27th edition, spells it without any apostrophe. It also specifies that the final S is silent, which implies that the possessive form would be "Capgras's", pronounced "kap-grahz".
  8. ^ Henriet K, Haouzir S, Petit M (March 2008). "L'illusion des sosies de Capgras : une interprétation délirante..". Annales Médico-psychologiques, revue psychiatrique 166 (2): 147–156. doi:10.1016/j.amp.2006.02.011. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6X0W-4MR1K7V-1&_user=10&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_sort=d&_docanchor=&view=c&_searchStrId=1151031716&_rerunOrigin=google&_acct=C000050221&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=10&md5=394929d04b18fc2314f53efc87c16583. 
  9. ^ Passer KM, Warnock JK (1991). "Pimozide in the treatment of Capgras' syndrome. A case report". Psychosomatics 32 (4): 446–8. doi:10.1016/S0033-3182(91)72049-5. PMID 1961860. http://psy.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=1961860. 
  10. ^ Bauer RM (1984). "Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: a neuropsychological application of the Guilty Knowledge Test". Neuropsychologia 22 (4): 457–69. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(84)90040-X. PMID 6483172. 
  11. ^ Ellis HD, Young AW (August 1990). "Accounting for delusional misidentifications". Br J Psychiatry 157 (2): 239–48. doi:10.1192/bjp.157.2.239. PMID 2224375. 
  12. ^ Ellis HD, Young AW, Quayle AH, De Pauw KW (1997). "Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion". Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 264 (1384): 1085–92. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0150. 
  13. ^ Hirstein W, Ramachandran VS (1997). "Capgras syndrome: a novel probe for understanding the neural representation of the identity and familiarity of persons". Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 264 (1380): 437–444. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0062. PMC 1688258. PMID 9107057. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1688258. 
  14. ^ Ramachandran, V. S; Blakeslee S. (1998). Phantoms in the Brain. Great Britain: Harper Perennial. ISBN 10-1-85702-895-3. 
  15. ^ Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio A (1995). "Double dissociation between overt and covert face recognition". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7 (4): 425–432. doi:10.1162/jocn.1995.7.4.425. 
  16. ^ Davies M, Coltheart M, Langdon R, Breen N (2001). "Monothematic delusions: Towards a two-factor account". Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 8 (2): 133–158. doi:10.1353/ppp.2001.0007. 

External links