Aphasia

Aphasia
Classification and external resources
ICD-10 F80.0-F80.2, R47.0
ICD-9 315.31, 784.3, 438.11
DiseasesDB 4024
MedlinePlus 003204
eMedicine neuro/437
MeSH D001037
Dysphasia
Classification and external resources
ICD-10 F80.1, F80.2, R47.0
ICD-9 438.12, 784.5

Aphasia (pronounced /əˈfeɪʒə/ or pronounced /əˈfeɪziə/) is an acquired language disorder in which there is an impairment of any language modality. This may include difficulty in producing or comprehending spoken or written language.

Traditionally, aphasia suggests the total impairment of language ability, and dysphasia a degree of impairment less than total. However, the term dysphasia is easily confused with dysphagia, a swallowing disorder, and thus aphasia has come to mean both partial and total language impairment in common use.

Depending on the area and extent of brain damage, someone suffering from aphasia may be able to speak but not write, or vice versa, or display any of a wide variety of other deficiencies in language comprehension and production, such as being able to sing but not speak. Aphasia may co-occur with speech disorders such as dysarthria or apraxia of speech, which also result from brain damage.

Aphasia can be assessed in a variety of ways, from quick clinical screening at the bedside to several-hour-long batteries of tasks that examine the key components of language and communication. The prognosis of those with aphasia varies widely, and is dependent upon age of the patient, site and size of lesion, and type of aphasia.

Contents

Classification

Classifying the different subtypes of aphasia is difficult and has led to disagreements among experts. The localizationist model is the original model, but modern anatomical techniques and analyses have shown that precise connections between brain regions and symptom classification don't exist. The neural organization of language is complicated; language is a comprehensive and complex behavior and it makes sense that it isn't the product of some small, circumscribed region of the brain.
No classification of patients in subtypes and groups of subtypes is adequate. Only about 60% of patients will fit in a classification scheme such as fluent/nonfluent/pure aphasias. There is a huge variation among patients with the same diagnosis, and aphasias can be highly selective. For instance, patients with naming deficits (anomic aphasia) might show an inability only for naming buildings, or people, or colors.[1]

Localizationist model

Cortex

The localizationist model attempts to classify the aphasia by major characteristics and then link these to areas of the brain in which the damage has been caused. The initial two categories here were devised by early neurologists working in the field, namely Paul Broca and Carl Wernicke. Other researchers have added to the model, resulting in it often being referred to as the "Boston-Neoclassical Model". The most prominent writers on this topic have been Harold Goodglass and Edith Kaplan.

  1. Pure word deafness
  2. Conduction aphasia
  3. Apraxia of speech, which is now considered a separate disorder in itself.
  4. Transcortical motor aphasia
  5. Transcortical sensory aphasia

Other ways to Classify Aphasia

Fluent, non-fluent and "pure" aphasias

The different types of aphasia can be divided into three categories: fluent, non-fluent and "pure" aphasias.[2]

Primary and secondary aphasia

Aphasia can be divided into primary and secondary aphasia.

Cognitive neuropsychological model

The cognitive neuropsychological model builds on cognitive neuropsychology. It assumes that language processing can be broken down into a number of modules, each of which has a specific function. Hence there is a module which recognises phonemes as they are spoken and a module which stores formulated phonemes before they are spoken. Use of this model clinically involves conducting a battery of assessments (usually from the PALPA), each of which tests one or a number of these modules. Once a diagnosis is reached as to where the impairment lies, therapy can proceed to treat the individual module.

Acquired childhood aphasia

Acquired childhood aphasia (ACA) is a language impairment resulting from some kind of brain damage. This brain damage can have different causes, such as head trauma, tumors, cerebrovascular accidents, or seizure disorders. Most, but not all authors state that ACA is preceded by a period of normal language development.[3] Age of onset is usually defined as from infancy until but not including adolescence.

ACA should be distinguished from developmental aphasia or developmental dysphasia, which is a primary delay or failure in language acquisition.[4] An important difference between ACA and developmental childhood aphasia is that in the latter there is no apparent neurological basis for the language deficit.[5]

ACA is one of the more rare language problems in children and is notable because of its contribution to theories on language and the brain.[4] Because there are so few children with ACA, not much is known about what types of linguistic problems these children have. However, many authors report a marked decrease in the use of all expressive language. Children can just stop talking for a period of weeks or even years, and when they start to talk again, they need a lot of encouragement. Problems with language comprehension are less common in ACA, and don't last as long.[6]

Signs and symptoms

People with aphasia may experience any of the following behaviors due to an acquired brain injury, although some of these symptoms may be due to related or concomitant problems such as dysarthria or apraxia and not primarily due to aphasia.

The following table summarizes some major characteristics of different types of aphasia:

Types of aphasia
Repetition Naming Auditory comprehension Fluency
Presentation
Wernicke's aphasia
mild–mod mild–severe defective fluent paraphasic
Individuals with Wernicke's aphasia may speak in long sentences that have no meaning, add unnecessary words, and even create new "words" (neologisms). For example, someone with Wernicke's aphasia may say, "You know that smoodle pinkered and that I want to get him round and take care of him like you want before", meaning "The dog needs to go out so I will take him for a walk". They have poor auditory and reading comprehension, and fluent, but nonsensical, oral and written expression. Individuals with Wernicke's aphasia usually have great difficulty understanding the speech of both themselves and others and are therefore often unaware of their mistakes.
Transcortical sensory aphasia
good mod–severe poor fluent
Similar deficits as in Wernicke's aphasia, but repetition ability remains intact.
Conduction aphasia
poor poor relatively good fluent
Conduction aphasia is caused by deficits in the connections between the speech-comprehension and speech-production areas. This might be damage to the arcuate fasciculus, the structure that transmits information between Wernicke's area and Broca's area. Similar symptoms, however, can be present after damage to the insula or to the auditory cortex. Auditory comprehension is near normal, and oral expression is fluent with occasional paraphasic errors. Repetition ability is poor.
Nominal or Anomic aphasia
mild mod–severe mild fluent
Anomic aphasia is essentially a difficulty with naming. The patient may have difficulties naming certain words, linked by their grammatical type (e.g. difficulty naming verbs and not nouns) or by their semantic category (e.g. difficulty naming words relating to photography but nothing else) or a more general naming difficulty. Patients tend to produce grammatic, yet empty, speech. Auditory comprehension tends to be preserved.
Broca's aphasia
mod–severe mod–severe mild difficulty non-fluent, effortful, slow
Individuals with Broca's aphasia frequently speak short, meaningful phrases that are produced with great effort. Broca's aphasia is thus characterized as a nonfluent aphasia. Affected people often omit small words such as "is", "and", and "the". For example, a person with Broca's aphasia may say, "Walk dog" which could mean "I will take the dog for a walk", "You take the dog for a walk" or even "The dog walked out of the yard". Individuals with Broca's aphasia are able to understand the speech of others to varying degrees. Because of this, they are often aware of their difficulties and can become easily frustrated by their speaking problems. It is associated with right hemiparesis, meaning that there can be paralysis of the patient's right face and arm.
Transcortical motor aphasia
good mild–severe mild non-fluent
Similar deficits as Broca's aphasia, except repetition ability remains intact. Auditory comprehension is generally fine for simple conversations, but declines rapidly for more complex conversations. It is associated with right hemiparesis, meaning that there can be paralysis of the patient's right face and arm.
Global aphasia
poor poor poor non-fluent
Individuals with global aphasia have severe communication difficulties and will be extremely limited in their ability to speak or comprehend language. They may be totally nonverbal, and/or only use facial expressions and gestures to communicate. It is associated with right hemiparesis, meaning that there can be paralysis of the patient's right face and arm.
Transcortical mixed aphasia
moderate poor poor non-fluent
Similar deficits as in global aphasia, but repetition ability remains intact.
Subcortical aphasias
 
Characteristics and symptoms depend upon the site and size of subcortical lesion. Possible sites of lesions include the thalamus, internal capsule, and basal ganglia.

Jargon aphasia is a fluent or receptive aphasia in which the patient's speech is incomprehensible, but appears to make sense to them. Speech is fluent and effortless with intact syntax and grammar, but the patient has problems with the selection of nouns. They will either replace the desired word with another that sounds or looks like the original one, or has some other connection, or they will replace it with sounds. Accordingly, patients with jargon aphasia often use neologisms, and may perseverate if they try to replace the words they can't find with sounds.

Commonly, substitutions involve picking another (actual) word starting with the same sound (e.g. clocktower - colander), picking another se

Causes

Aphasia usually results from lesions to the language-relevant areas of the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes of the brain, such as Broca's area, Wernicke's area, and the neural pathways between them. These areas are almost always located in the left hemisphere, and in most people this is where the ability to produce and comprehend language is found. However, in a very small number of people, language ability is found in the right hemisphere. In either case, damage to these language areas can be caused by a stroke, traumatic brain injury, or other brain injury. Aphasia may also develop slowly, as in the case of a brain tumor or progressive neurological disease, e.g., Alzheimer's or Parkinson's disease. It may also be caused by a sudden hemorrhagic event within the brain. Certain chronic neurological disorders, such as epilepsy or migraine, can also include transient aphasia as a prodromal or episodic symptom. Aphasia is also listed as a rare side effect of the fentanyl patch, an opioid used to control chronic pain.[7]

Treatment

There is no one treatment proven to be effective for all types of aphasias. Melodic intonation therapy is often used to treat non-fluent aphasia and has proved to be very effective in some cases.

History

The first recorded case of aphasia is from an Egyptian papyrus, the Edwin Smith Papyrus, which details speech problems in a person with a traumatic brain injury to the temporal lobe.[8]

Notable cases

See also

Notes

  1. Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2003). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. [New York]: Worth. pp. 502, 505, 511. ISBN 0-7167-5300-6. OCLC 464808209. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2003). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. [New York]: Worth. pp. 502-504. ISBN 0-7167-5300-6. OCLC 464808209. 
  3. Murdoch, B. E. (1990). Acquired neurological speech/language disorders in childhood. Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 0-85066-490-X. OCLC 21976166. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 Woods, Bryan T. (1995). "Acquired childhood aphasia". In Kirshner, Howard S.. Handbook of neurological speech and language disorders. New York: M. Dekker. ISBN 0-8247-9282-3. OCLC 31075598. 
  5. Paquier, P.F.; van Dongen (1998). "Is Acquired Childhood Aphasia Atypical?". In Basso, Anna; Coppens, Patrick; Lebrun, Yvan. Aphasia in atypical populations. Hillsdale, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-1738-7. OCLC 37712996. 
  6. Baker, Lorian; Cantwell, Dennis P. (1987). Developmental speech and language disorders. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 0-89862-400-2. OCLC 14520470. 
  7. "Fentanyl Transdermal Official FDA information, side effects and uses.". Drug Information Online. http://www.drugs.com/pro/fentanyl-transdermal.html#A02A9CB6-35CF-4F01-A980-C3733E0F861A. 
  8. McCrory PR, Berkovic SF (December 2001). "Concussion: the history of clinical and pathophysiological concepts and misconceptions". Neurology 57 (12): 2283–9. PMID 11756611. http://www.neurology.org/cgi/content/abstract/57/12/2283. 
  9. Richardson, Robert G. (1995). Emerson: the mind on fire: a biography. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 0-520-08808-5. OCLC 31206668. 

References

Handbooks

Bibliographic Databases

Specialized Bibliographies

Academic references

  • Dr. Kalina Christoff, The Cognitive Neuroscience of Thought Laboratory publication
  • Chapey, Roberta (2008). Language intervention strategies in aphasia and related neurogenic communication disorders. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 0-7817-6981-7. OCLC 173201745. 
  • Barresi, Barbara; Goodglass, Harold; Kaplan, Edith (2001). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 0-683-03604-1. OCLC 43650748. 
  • Coltheart, Max; Kay, Janice; Lesser, Ruth (1992). PALPA psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia. Hillsdale, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-86377-166-1. OCLC 221303581. 
  • Risser, Anthony H.; Spreen, Otfried (2003). Assessment of aphasia. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-514075-3. OCLC 474049850. 
  • Jürgen Tesak; Christopher Code (2008). Milestones in the History of Aphasia: Theories and Protagonists (Brain Damage, Behaviour, and Cognition). East Sussex: Psychology Press. ISBN 1-84169-513-0. OCLC 165957752. 
  • Leonard L., PhD. Lapointe (2004). Aphasia And Related Neurogenic Language Disorders. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers. ISBN 1-58890-226-9. OCLC 57071469. 
  • Byng, Sally; Duchan, Judith F.; Felson Duchan, Judith (2004). Challenging Aphasia Therapies: Broadening the Discourse and Extending the Boundaries. East Sussex: Psychology Press. ISBN 1-84169-505-X. OCLC 473789757. 
  • De Bleser, Ria; Papathanasiou, Ilias (2003). The sciences of aphasia from therapy to theory. Amsterdam: Pergamon. ISBN 0-585-47448-6. OCLC 53277297. 

Personal experiences of aphasia

  • Sheila Hale (2002). The man who lost his language. London: Allen Lane. ISBN 0-7139-9361-8. OCLC 50099900. 
  • Stephanie Mensh; Berger, Paul D.; Whitaker, Julian M. (2002). How to conquer the world with one hand-- and an attitude. Positive Power Pub. ISBN 0-9668378-7-8. OCLC 52445790. 
  • Cindy Greatrex (2005) Aphasia in the Deaf Community.
  • Dardick, Geeta (1991), Prisoner of Silence, Reader's Digest, June issue