Pattern theory
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
- For other meanings of "pattern", see Pattern (disambiguation).
Pattern theory, formulated by Ulf Grenander, is a mathematical formalism to describe knowledge of the world as patterns. It differs from other approaches to artificial intelligence in that it does not begin by prescribing algorithms and machinery to recognize and classify patterns; rather, it prescribes a vocabulary to articulate and recast the pattern concepts in precise language.
In addition to the new algebraic vocabulary, its statistical approach was novel in its aim to:
- Identify the hidden variables of a data set using real world data rather than artificial stimuli, which was commonplace at the time.
- Formulate prior distributions for hidden variables and models for the observed variables that form the vertices of a Gibbs-like graph.
- Study the randomness and variability of these graphs.
- Create the basic classes of stochastic models applied by listing the deformations of the patterns.
- Synthesize (sample) from the models, not just analyze signals with it.
Broad in its mathematical coverage, Pattern Theory spans algebra and statistics, as well as local topological and global entropic properties.
The Brown University Pattern Theory Group was formed in 1972 by Ulf Grenander. Many mathematicians are currently working in this group, noteworthy among them being the Fields Medalist David Mumford. Mumford regards Grenander as his "guru" in this subject.
Contents |
[edit] Algebraic foundations
We begin with an example to motivate the algebraic definitions that follows.
- Example 1 Grammar
|
As one can tell from this example, and typical of signals we study, identifying the primitives and bond tables require some thought. The example highlights another important fact not readily apparent in other signals modalities: that a configuration is not the signal we observe; rather, we observe its image as a sentence. Herein lies a significant justification to distinguish an observable from a non-observable construct. Additionally, it gives us an algebraic structure to associate our Hidden Markov Models with. In sensory modalities such as the vision example below, the hidden configurations and observed images are much more similar that such a distinction may not seem justified. Fortunately, we have the Grammar example to remind us of this distinction.
Motivated by the example, we have the following definitions:
1. A generator , drawn as
is the primitive of Pattern Theory that generates the observed signal. Structurally, it is a value with interfaces, called bonds , which connects the g's to form a signal generator. 2 neighboring generators are connected when their bond values are the same. Similarity self-maps s: G -> G express the invariances of the world we are looking at, such as rigid body transformations, or scaling.
2. Bonds glue generators into a configuration, c, which creates the signal against a backdrop Σ, with global features described locally by a bond coupling table called ρ. The boolean function ρ is the principal component of the regularity 4-tuple <G,S,ρ,Σ>, which is defined as
Regularity is designed to capture the notion of the global feature of interest on a local scale.
3. An image (C mod R) captures the notion of an observed Configuration, as opposed to one which exists independently from any perceptual apparatus. Images are configurations distinguished only by their external bonds, inheriting the configuration’s composition and similarities transformations. Formally, images are equivalence classes partitioned by an Identification Rule "~" with 3 properties:
- ext(c) = ext(c') whenever c~c'
- sc~sc' whenever c~c'
- sigma(c1,c2) ~ sigma(c1',c2') whenever c1~c1', c2~c2' are all regular.
A configuration corresponding to a physical stimulus may have many images, corresponding to many observer perception's identification rule.
4. A pattern is the repeatable components of an image, defined as the S-invariant subset of an image. Similarities are reference transformations we use to define patterns, eg. rigid body transformations. At first glance, this definition seems suited for only texture patterns where the minimal sub-image is repeated over and over again. If we were to view an image of an object such as a dog, its is not repeated, yet seem like it seems familiar and should be a pattern. (Help needed here).
5. A deformation is a transformation of the original image that accounts for the noise in the environment and error in the perceptual apparatus. Grenander identifies 4 types of deformations: noise and blur, multi-scale superposition, domain warping, and interruptions.
- Example 2 Directed boundary
|
With the benefit of being given generators and complete bond tables, a difficult part of pattern analysis is done. In tackling a new class of signals and features, the task of devising the generators and bond table is much more difficult
Again, just as in grammars, identifying the generators and bond tables require some thought. Just as subtle is the fact that a configuration is not the signal we observe. Rather, we observe its image as silhouette projections of the identification rule.
[edit] Topology
to be expanded !
[edit] Entropy
PT defines order in terms of the feature of interest given by p(c).
- Energy(c) = −log P(c)
[edit] Statistics
Grenander’s Pattern Theory treatment of Bayesian inference in seems to be skewed towards on image reconstruction (eg. content addressable memory). That is given image I-deformed, find I. However, Mumford’s interpretation of Pattern Theory is broader and he defines PT to include many more well-known statistical methods. Mumford’s criteria for inclusion of a topic as Pattern Theory are those methods "characterized by common techniques and motivations", such as the HMM, EM algorithm, dynamic programming circle of ideas. Topics in this section will reflect Mumford's treatment of Pattern Theory. His principle of statistical Pattern Theory are the following:
- Use real world signals rather than constructed ones to infer the hidden states of interest.
- Such signals contain too much complexity and artifacts to succumb to a purely deterministic analysis, so employ stochastic methods too.
- Respect the natural structure of the signal, including any symmetries, independence of parts, marginals on key statistics. Validate by sampling from the derived models by and infer hidden states with Bayes’ rule.
- Across all modalities, a limited family of deformations distort the pure patterns into real world signals.
- Stochastic factors affecting an observation show strong conditional independence.
Statistical PT makes ubiquitous use of conditional probability in the form of Bayes theorem and Markov Models. Both these concepts are used to express the relation between hidden states (configurations) and observed states (images). Markov Models also captures the local properties of the stimulus, reminiscent of the purpose of bond table for regularity.
The generic set up is the following: Let s = the hidden state of the data that we wish to know. i = observed image. Bayes theorem gives
-
- p (s | i ) p(i) = p (s, i ) = p (i|s ) p(s)
- To analyze the signal (recognition): fix i, maximize p, infer s.
- To synthesize signals (sampling): fix s, generate i's, compare w/ real world images
The following conditional probability examples illustrates these methods in action:
[edit] Conditional probability for local properties
N-gram Text Strings: See Mumford's Pattern Theory by Examples, Chapter 1.
MAP ~ MDL (MDL offers a glimpse of why the MAP probabilistic formulation make sense analytically)
[edit] Conditional probability for hidden-observed states
Bayes Theorem for Machine translation |
Supposing we want to translate French sentences to English. Here, the hidden configurations are English sentences and the observed signal they generate are French sentences. Bayes theorem gives p(e|f)p(f) = p(e, f) = p(f|e)p(e) and reduces to the fundamental equation of machine translation: maximize p(e|f) = p(f|e)p(e) over the appropriate e (note that p(f) is independent of e, and so drops out when we maximize over e). This reduces the problem to three main calculations for:
The analysis seems to be symmetric with respect to the two languages, and if we think can calculate p(f|e), why not turn the analysis around and calculate p(e|f) directly? The reason is that during the calculation of p(f|e) the asymmetric assumption is made that source sentence be well formed and we cannot make any such assumption about the target translation because we do not know what it will translate into. We now focus on p(f|e) in the three-part decomposition above. The other two parts, p(e) and maximizing e, uses similar techniques as the N-gram model. Given a French-English translation from a large training data set (such data sets exists from the Canadian parliament), NULL And the program has been implemented Le programme a ete mis en application the sentence pair can be encoded as an alignment (2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 6, 6) that reads as follows: the first word in French comes from the second English word, the second word in French comes from the 3rd English word, and so forth. Although an alignment is a straight forward encoding of the translation, a more computationally convenient approach to an alignment is to break it down into four parameters:
p(f|e ) = Sum over all possible alignments an of p(a, f | e ) = For the sake of simplicity in demonstrating an EM algorithm, we shall go through a simple calculation involving only translation probabilities t(), but needless to say that it the method applies to all parameters in their full glory. Consider the simplified case (1) without the NULL word (2) where every word has fertility 1 and (3) there are no distortion probabilities. Our training data corpus will contain two-sentence pairs: bc → xy and b → y. The translation of a two-word English sentence “b c” into the French sentence “x y” has two possible alignments, and including the one-sentence words, the alignments are: b c b c b | | x | x y x y y called Parallel, Crossed, and Singleton respectively. To illustrate an EM algorithm, first set the desired parameter uniformly, that is
Then EM iterates as follows The alignment probability for the “crossing alignment” (where b connects to y) got a boost from the second sentence pair b/y. That further solidified t(y | b), but as a side effect also boosted t(x | c), because x connects to c in that same “crossing alignment.” The effect of boosting t(x | c) necessarily means downgrading t(y | c) because they sum to one. So, even though y and c co-occur, analysis reveals that they are not translations of each other. With real data, EM also is subject to the usual local extremum traps. |
HMM’s for speech recognition |
For decades, speech recognition seemed to hit an impasse as scientists sought descriptive and analytic solution. The sound wave p(t) below is produced by speaking the word “ski”. Its four distinct segments has very different characteristics. One can choose from many levels of generators (hidden variables): the intention of the speaker’s brain, the state of the mouth and vocal cords, or the ‘phones’ themselves. Phones are the generator of choice to be inferred and it encodes the word in a noisy, highly variable way. Early work on speech recognition attempted to make this inference deterministically using logical rules based on binary features extracted from p(t). For instance, the table below shows some of the features used to distinguish English consonants. In real situations, the signal is further complicated by background noises such as cars driving by or artifacts such as a cough in mid sentence (mumford’s 2nd underpinning). The deterministic rule-based approach failed and the state of the art (eg. Dragon Naturally Speaking) is to use a family of precisely tuned HMM’s and a Bayesian MAP estimators to do better. Similar stories played out in vision, and other stimulus categories. (See Mumford's Pattern Theory: the mathematics of perception) The Markov stochastic process is diagrammed as follows: exponentials, EM algorithm |
[edit] Further reading
- 2007. Ulf Grenander and Michael Miller Pattern Theory: From Representation to Inference. Oxford University Press. Paperback. (ISBN 9780199297061)
- 1994. Ulf Grenander General Pattern Theory. Oxford Science Publications. (ISBN 978-0198536710)
- 1996. Ulf Grenander Elements of Pattern Theory. Johns Hopkins University Press. (ISBN 978-0801851889)