Imaginary color

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

The CIE 1931 color space chromaticity diagram. The white regions correspond to imaginary colors.
The CIE 1931 color space chromaticity diagram. The white regions correspond to imaginary colors.

Non-physical, unrealizable, or imaginary colors are combinations of cone cell responses that cannot be produced by any physical light source.[1] Thus, no object can have an imaginary color, and imaginary colors cannot be seen under normal circumstances. Nevertheless, they are useful as mathematical abstractions for defining color spaces.

Normalized wavelength absorption of pigments in the three kinds of cones, and rods.
Normalized wavelength absorption of pigments in the three kinds of cones, and rods.

For example, the wavelength response curve of medium-wavelength ("green") cone cells overlaps those of both short-wavelength ("blue") and long-wavelength ("red") cone cells. Light of any wavelength that interacts with green cones also interacts with blue and red cones to some extent. Therefore, there is no spectral power distribution that excites only green cones without exciting blue or red cones at all, and that situation corresponds to an imaginary color greener than any physical green.

Contents

[edit] Concept and utility

The ProPhoto RGB color space uses imaginary green and blue primaries to obtain a larger gamut (space inside the triangle) than would be possible with three real primaries. However, some real colors are still irreproducible.
The ProPhoto RGB color space uses imaginary green and blue primaries to obtain a larger gamut (space inside the triangle) than would be possible with three real primaries. However, some real colors are still irreproducible.

Real colors are colors that can be produced by a physical light source. Any additive mixture of two real colors is also a real color. When colors are displayed in the CIE 1931 XYZ color space, additive mixture results in a color along the line between the colors being mixed. By mixing any three colors, one can therefore create colors in the triangle between the three colors—this is called the gamut formed by those three colors, which are called primary colors. Any colors outside of this triangle can not be obtained.

When defining primaries, the goal is often to leave as many real colors in gamut as possible. Since the region of real colors is not a triangle (see illustration), it is not possible to pick three real colors that span the whole region. It is possible to increase the gamut by selecting more than three real primary colors, but since the region of real colors is not a polygon, there always will be some colors at the edge left out. Therefore, one selects colors outside of the region of real colors as primary colors; in other words, imaginary primary colors. Mathematically, the gamut created in this way contains so-called “imaginary colors”.

[edit] Perception of imaginary colors

If a saturated green is viewed until the green receptors are fatigued and then a saturated red is viewed, a perception of red more intense than pure spectral red can be experienced. This is due to the fatigue of the green receptors and the resulting lack of their ability to desaturate the perceptual response to the output of the red receptors.[2]

[edit] References

  1. ^ MacEvoy, Bruce (2005). Light and the eye. Handprint. Retrieved on 2007-05-20.
  2. ^ Lindsay, Peter and Norman, Donald, "Human Information Processing," Academic Press, 1972, pp 196–216.

[edit] See also