Hockney-Falco thesis
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
The Hockney-Falco thesis is a controversial theory of art history, advanced by artist David Hockney and physicist Charles M. Falco, suggesting that advances in realism and accuracy in the history of Western art since the Renaissance were primarily the result of optical aids such as the camera obscura, camera lucida, and curved mirrors, rather than the development of artistic technique and skill. In a 2001 book, Secret Knowledge: Rediscovering the Lost Techniques of the Old Masters, Hockney analyzed the work of the Old Masters and argued that the level of accuracy represented in their work is impossible to create by "eyeballing it". Since then, Hockney and Falco have produced a number of publications on positive evidence of the use optical aids, and the historical plausibility of such methods.
Part of Hockney's work involved collaboration with Charles Falco, a condensed matter physicist and an expert in optics. While the use of optical aids would generally enhance accuracy, Falco calculated the types of distortion that would result from specific optical devices; Hockney and Falco argued that such errors could in fact be found in the work of some of the Old Masters.[1]
Hockney's book prompted intense and sustained debate among artists, art historians, and a wide variety of other scholars. In particular, it has spurred increased interest in the actual methods and techniques of artists, among scientists and historians of science, as well as general historians and art historians. Art historians have in general reacted unfavorably, interpreting the Hockney-Falco thesis as an accusation that the Old Masters "cheated" and intentionally obscured their methods.[2] Physicist David G. Stork and several co-authors have argued against the Hockney-Falco thesis from a technical standpoint.[3][4][5]
Contents |
[edit] Origins of the thesis
As described in Secret Knowledge, in January 1999 during a visit to the National Gallery, London Hockney conceived of the idea that optical aids were the key factor in the development of artistic realism. He was struck by the accuracy of portraits by Jean Auguste Dominique Ingres, and became convinced that Ingres had used a camera lucida or similar device. From there, Hockney began looking for signs of the use of optical aids in earlier paintings, creating what he called the Great Wall in his studio by organizing images of great realistic art by time period. What he saw as a sudden rise of realism around 1420, combined with Charles Falco's suggestion that concave mirrors could have been used in that period to project images, was the germ of the Hockney-Falco thesis.[6]
In 2000, Falco and Hockney published an analysis ("Optical Insights into Renaissance Art") of the likely use of concave mirrors in Jan Van Eyck's work in Optics & Photonics News, vol. 11. In 2001, Hockney published an extended form of his argument in Secret Knowledge.
[edit] Hockney's argument
In Secret Knowledge, Hockney argues that early Renaissance artists such as Jan Van Eyck and Lorenzo Lotto used concave mirrors; as evidence, he points to the chandelier in Van Eyck's Arnolfini Portrait, the ear in Van Eyck's portrait of Cardinal Albergati, and the carpet in Lotto's Husband and Wife. Hockney suggests that later artists, beginning with Caravaggio, used convex mirrors as well, to achieve a large field of view.
[edit] Falco and Ibn al-Haytham
At a scientific conference in February 2007, Falco further argued that the Iraqi Arab Muslim physicist Ibn al-Haytham's (965–1040) work on optics, in his Book of Optics, may have influenced the use of optical aids by Renaissance artists. Falco said that his and Hockney's examples of Renaissance art "demonstrate a continuum in the use of optics by artists from c. 1430, arguably initiated as a result of Ibn al-Haytham's influence, until today."[7]
[edit] Criticism
Some art historians have criticized Hockney's argument on the grounds that the use of optical aids, though well-established in individual cases, has little value for explaining the overall development of Western art, and that historical records and paintings and photographs of art studios (sans optical devices), as well as present-day realist artists, demonstrate that high levels of realism are possible without optical aids.[8] In addition to incredulity on the part of art historians, some of the harshest criticism of the Hockney-Falco thesis came from another expert in optics, image processing and pattern recognition David G. Stork. Stork analyzed the images used by Falco and Hockney, and came to the conclusion that they do not demonstrate the kinds of optical distortion that curved mirrors or converging lenses would cause.[9] However, Falco has claimed that Stork's published criticisms have relied on fabricated data and misrepresentations of Hockney and Falco's theory [10], although Stork has rebutted this claim.[11]
Leaving the technical optical arguments aside, historians of science investigated several aspects of the historical plausibility of the thesis in a 2005 set of articles in Early Science and Medicine. In his introduction to the volume, Sven Dupré claimed the Hockney-Falco analysis rests heavily on a small number of examples, "a few dozen square centimeters" of canvas that seem to show signs that optical devices were used.[12] Sara J. Schechner claimed that surviving glassware from the 15th and 16th centuries is far too imperfect to have been used to create realistic images, while "even thinking about projecting images was alien to the contemporary conceptual frame of mind."[13] Another scholar argued that the optical ideas of opticians at the time were somewhat incompatible with image projection, though artists might have been more receptive. Two other scholars showed that Renaissance painting treatises and Leonardo's manuscripts also lack any reference to image projection. The conclusion based on these studies was that "the Hockney-Falco thesis [is] extremely unlikely as far as its application for the period before the first textual reference to image projection around 1550 is concerned." However, the historians were more sanguine about the possible relevance of the thesis between 1550 and the invention of the telescope, and cautiously supportive after that period, when there clearly was interest and capacity to project realistic images; 17th century painters such as Johannes Vermeer and Gaspar van Wittel used optical devices in variety of ways, though not the ways postulated by Hockney.[14]
Vincent Ilardi, a historian of Renaissance optical glass, subsequently argued against Schechner's conclusions based on surviving glassware, suggesting that the present condition of Renaissance glassware is not likely to reflect the optical quality of such glassware when it was new. Ilardi documents Lorenzo Lotto's purchase of a high-priced crystal mirror in 1549, bolstering the Hockney-Falco thesis in Lotto's case.[15] On his website, Falco also claims Schechner overlooked manuscript evidence for the use of mirrors made from steel and other metals (which would have quickly corroded to the point of uselessness without maintenance), as well as numerous metal artifacts that belie the claim that sufficiently large and reflective metal mirrors were unavailable, and that other contributors to the Early Science and Medicine volume relied on Schechner's mistaken work in dismissing the thesis.[16]
[edit] References
- ^ Charles Falco, FAQ (accessed March 16, 2007)
- ^ Sven Dupré, "Introduction. The Hockney-Falco Thesis: Constraints and Opportunities", Early Science and Medicine, vol. 10, issue 2 (2005), pp. 125-126
- ^ Antonio Criminisi and David G. Stork, "Did the great masters use optical projections while painting? Perspective comparison of paintings and photographs of Renaissance chandeliers," in J. Kittler, M. Petrou and M. S. Nixon (eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Pattern Recognition, Volume IV, pp. 645-648, 2004
- ^ David G. Stork, "Optics and realism in Renaissance art," Scientific American 291(6):76-84, December 2004 [1]
- ^ Christopher W. Tyler, "Rosetta Stoned?" http://www.diatrope.com/hockney.html
- ^ Sven Dupré, "Introduction. The Hockney-Falco Thesis: Constraints and Opportunities", Early Science and Medicine, vol. 10, issue 2 (2005), pp. 126-127
- ^ Falco, Charles M. "Ibn al-Haytham and the Origins of Modern Image Analysis" presented at a plenary session at the International Conference on Information Sciences, Signal Processing and its Applications, 12–15 February 2007. Sharjah, United Arab Emirates (U.A.E.). [2]
- ^ David K. Yoder, Why David Hockney Should Not Be Taken Seriously (accessed March 16, 2007)
- ^ David G. Stork, FAQ (accessed March 16, 2007)
- ^ Rex Dalton, "Tempers blaze over artistic integrity", Nature, Vol. 440, No. 7081 (March 9, 2006), p. 134
- ^ David G. Stork and Marco Duarte, "Revisiting computer vision and art," "IEEE Multimedia" Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 108-109, 2007 and http://www.diatrope.com/stork/HockneyFAQs.html
- ^ Sven Dupré, "Introduction. The Hockney-Falco Thesis: Constraints and Opportunities", Early Science and Medicine, vol. 10, issue 2 (2005), pp. 126-127; quotation from p. 127
- ^ Sara J. Schechner, "Between Knowing and Doing: Mirrors and their Imperfections in the Renaissance", Early Science and Medicine, vol. 10, issue 2 (2005), pp. 137-162
- ^ Sven Dupré, "Introduction. The Hockney-Falco Thesis: Constraints and Opportunities", Early Science and Medicine, vol. 10, issue 2 (2005), pp. 128-135; quotation from p. 131
- ^ Vincent Ilardi, Renaissance Vision, From Spectacles to Telescopes (Memoirs of the American Philosophical Society), American Philosophical Society, 2007, pp. 197-199. ISBN 0871692597
- ^ Charles Falco, FAQ: Technical and Historical Support for, and Objections to, our Thesis (accessed April 2, 2008)
[edit] External links
- FAQ by Charles Falco - a summary of the physical and historical evidence
- FAQ by David G. Stork - another physicist's response to Hockney-Falco thesis