Cultural memory
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
For other approaches see Memory (disambiguation) and Culture (disambiguation)
As a term, cultural memory was first introduced by the German Egyptologists Jan Assmann and Aleida Assmann, who drew further upon Maurice Halbwachs’s theory on collective memory. Both Jan Assmann and more present-day scholars like Andreas Huyssen have identified a general interest in memory and mnemonics since the early 1980’s, illustrated by phenomena as diverse as memorials and retro-culture. Some might see cultural memory as becoming more democratic, due to liberalization and the rise of new media. Others see cultural memory as remaining concentrated in the hands of corporations and states.
Because memory is not just an individual, private experience but is also part of the collective domain, cultural memory has become a topic in both historiography (Pierre Nora, Richard Terdiman) and cultural studies (e.g. Susan Stewart). These emphasize cultural memory’s process (historiography) and its implications and objects (cultural studies), respectively. Memory is a phenomenon that is directly related to the present; our perception of the past is always influenced by the present, which means that it is always changing.
Contents |
[edit] Historiographical approach
[edit] Time
Crucial in understanding cultural memory as a phenomenon is the distinction between memory and history. This distinction was put forward by Pierre Nora, who pinpointed a niche in-between history and memory. Simply put, memories are the events that actually happened, while histories are subjective representations of what historians believe is crucial to remember. This dichotomy, it should be noted, emerged at a particular moment in history: it implies that there used to be a time when memories could exist as such – without being representational.
Scholars disagree as to when to locate the moment representation 'took over'. Nora points to the formation of European nation states. For Richard Terdiman, the French revolution is the breaking point: the change of a political system, together with the emergence of industrialization and urbanization, made life more complex than ever before. This not only resulted in an increasing difficulty for people to understand the new society in which they were living, but also, as this break was so radical, people had trouble relating to the past before the revolution. In this situation, people no longer had an implicit understanding of their past. In order to understand the past, it had to be represented through history. As people realized that history was only one version of the past, they became more and more concerned with their own cultural heritage (in French called patrimoine) which helped them shape a collective and national identity. In search for an identity to bind a country or people together, governments have constructed collective memories in the form of commemorations which should bring and keep together minority groups and individuals with conflicting agendas. What becomes clear is that the obsession with memory coincides with the fear of forgetting and the aim for authenticity.
However, more recently questions have arisen whether there ever was a time in which 'pure', non-representational memory existed – as Nora in particular put forward. Scholars like Tony Bennett rightly point out that representation is a crucial precondition for human perception in general: pure, organic and objective memories can never be witnessed as such.
[edit] Space
It is because of a sometimes too contracted conception of memory as just a temporal phenomenon, that the concept of cultural memory has often been exposed to misunderstanding. Nora pioneered connecting memory to physical, tangible locations, nowadays globally known and incorporated as lieux de mémoire. He certifies these in his work as mises en abîme; entities that symbolize a more complex piece of our history. Although he concentrates on a spatial approach to remembrance, Nora already points out in his early historiographical theories that memory goes beyond just tangible and visual aspects, thereby making it flexible and in flux. This rather problematic notion, also characterized by Terdiman as the 'omnipresence' of memory, implies that for instance on a sensory level, a smell or a sound can become of cultural value, due to its commemorative effect.
Either in visualized or abstracted form, one of the largest complications of memorializing our past is the inevitable fact that it is absent. Every memory we try to reproduce becomes – as Terdiman states – a 'present past'. It is this impractical desire for recalling what is gone forever that brings to surface a feeling of nostalgia, noticeable in many aspects of daily life but most specifically in cultural products.
[edit] Cultural Studies approach
[edit] Embodied Memory
Recently, interest has developed in the area of 'embodied memory'. According to Paul Connerton the body can also be seen as a container, or carrier of memory, of two different types of social practice; inscribing and incorporating. The former includes all activities which are helpful for storing and retrieving information: photographing, writing, taping, etc. The latter implies skilled performances which are sent by means of physical activity, like a spoken word or a handshake. These performances are accomplished by the individual in an unconscious manner, and one might suggest that this memory carried in gestures and habits, is more authentic than 'indirect' memory via inscribing.
The first conceptions of embodied memory, in which the past is 'situated' in the body of the individual, derive from late nineteenth century thoughts of evolutionists like Jean Baptiste Lamarck and Ernst Haeckel. Lamarck’s law of inheritance of acquired characteristics and Haeckel’s theory of ontogeny recapitulating phylogeny, suggested that the individual is a summation of the whole history that had preceded him or her.
[edit] Objects
Memory can, for instance, be contained in objects. Souvenirs and photographs inhabit an important place in the cultural memory discourse. Several authors stress the fact that the relationship between memory and objects has changed since the nineteenth century. Stewart, for example, claims that our culture has changed from a culture of production to a culture of consumption. Products, according to Terdiman, have lost 'the memory of their own process' now, in times of mass-production and commodification. At the same time, he claims, the connection between memories and objects has been institutionalized and exploited in the form of trade in souvenirs. These specific objects can refer to either a distant time (an antique) or a distant (exotic) place. Stewart explains how our souvenirs authenticate our experiences and how they are a survival sign of events that exist only through the invention of narrative.
This notion can easily be applied to another practice that has a specific relationship with memory: photography. Catherine Keenan explains how the act of taking a picture can underline the importance of remembering, both individually and collectively. Also she states that pictures cannot only stimulate or help memory, but can rather eclipse the actual memory – when we remember in terms of the photograph – or they can serve as a reminder of our propensity to forget. Others have argued that photographs can be incorporated in memory and therefore supplement it.
[edit] Between Culture and Memory: Experience
As a contrast to the sometimes generative nature of previously mentioned studies on cultural memory, an alternative 'school' with its origins in gender and postcolonial studies underscored the importance of the individual and particular memories of those unheard in most collective accounts: women, minorities, homosexuals, etc.
Experience, whether it be lived or imagined, relates mutually to culture and memory. It is influenced by both factors, but determines these at the same time. Culture influences experience by offering mediated perceptions that affect it, as Frigga Haug states by opposing conventional theory on femininity to lived memory. In turn, experience affects culture, since individual experience becomes communicable and therefore collective. A memorial, for example, can represent a shared sense of loss.
The influence of memory is made obvious in the way the past is experienced in present conditions, for – according to Paul Connerton, for instance, – it can never be eliminated from human practice. On the other hand, it is perception driven by a longing for authenticity that colors memory, which is made clear by a desire to experience the real (Susan Stewart). Experience, therefore, is substantial to the interpretation of culture as well as memory, and vice versa.
[edit] References
- Assmann, J. (1992) Das Kulturelle Gedächtnis: Schrift, Erinnerung und Politische Identität in frühen Hochkulturen. Munich: Verlag C.H. Beck
- Bennet, T. (2006) 'Stored Virtue: Memory, the Body and the Evolutionary Museum', Memory Cultures: Memory, Subjectivity and Recognition. New Brunswick & London; Transaction Publishers, 40-54.
- Connerton, P. (1989) Bodily Practices. How Societies Remember. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 72-104.
- Halbwachs, M. (1950) La Mémoire Collective. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
- Haeckel, E. (1883) The Evolution of Man. http://www.gutenberg.org/etext/8700 (October 23 2006)
- Haug, F. (1987) Memory Work. Female Sexualization: A Collective Work of Memory. London: Verso, 33-72.
- Hirsch, M. (2002) 'Pictures of a Displaced Childhood', Family Frames: Photography, Narrative and Postmemory, 217-240.
- Huyssen, A. (2000) 'Present Pasts: Media, Politics, Amnesia', Public Culture 12:1, 21-38.
- Keenan, C. (1998) 'On the Relationship between Personal Photographs and Individual Memory', History of Photography 22:1, 60-64.
- Lamarck, J-P. (1984) Zoological Philosophy: An Exposition With Regard to the Natural History of Animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
- Nora, P. (1989) 'Between Memory and History: Les Lieux de Mémoire', Representations 26, 7-25.
- Nora, P. (1996) 'The Era of Commemoration', Pierre Nora & L. Kritzman (eds.) Realms of Memory: The construction of the French Past Vol. 3. New York: Columbia University Press, 609-637.
- Nora, P. (2002) 'The Reasons for the Current Upsurge in Memory', Transit – Europäische Revue 22. http://www.iwm.at/index.php?option=com_content&task=view&id=285&Itemid=463
- Steedman, C. (1986) Landscape for a good woman. London: Virago.
- Stewart, S. (1993) 'Objects of desire. Part I: The Souvenir', On Longing: Narratives of the Miniature, the Gigantic, the Souvenir, the Collection. Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 132-151.
- Sturken, M. (1999) 'The Image as Memorial: Personal Photographs in Cultural Memory', Marianne Hirsch (ed.) The Familial Gaze. Lebanon, NH: University Press of New England, 178-195.
- Terdiman, R. (1993) 'Historicizing Memory', Present Past: Modernity and the Memory Crisis. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 3-32.
[edit] See also
- Cultural history
- Culture industry
- History
- Identity
- Philosophy of history
- Popular culture studies
- Representation
- Roland Barthes
- Visual culture
[edit] External links
- Memory Studies at McGill University, ON Canada
- Christian Hubert on memory (choose m, memory)
- Center for Research on Social Cultural Change, Manchester UK
- [1] Producing National Identity by John Petrovato. Published in Stateofnature.org