Cognitive advantages to bilingualism
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
This article does not cite any references or sources. (March 2007) Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources. Unverifiable material may be challenged and removed. |
The neutrality or factuality of this article or section may be compromised by unattributed statements. You can help Wikipedia by removing weasel worded statements. |
There are some cognitive advantages to multilingualism.
Contents |
[edit] Old misconceptions
It was once thought[weasel words] that learning two languages was detrimental to a child's cognitive abilities.[citation needed] This was due to the idea that the two languages were learned independently and the knowledge of learning one did not transfer into the other. It was thought that as more was learned in one language, less could be learned in the other. This gives the idea of there being a total amount of language acquisition, and so the pieces learned in each language together have to add up to this total. For this reason parents and teachers tried to force children to only learn one language instead of cultivating the ability to learn both.[citation needed]
The consensus among linguists, as well as the general public today, however, is leaning towards the opposite; the idea that knowledge in the two languages would be kept separate instead of influencing each other is rejected as irrational by many. For example, a child who has learned the concept of adding and subtracting in one language would not need to re-learn the concept in another language. By that same token, a child who has learned to recognize that spoken language can be broken up into words, which can be represented in writing in one language is not going to need to be re-taught the idea of writing representing spoken language.
The idea that a person's language ability in one language is impaired by possession of facility in another has been subject to considerable debate.[citation needed]
[edit] Enhancing effects in children
When a child is fluent in two languages, they know more than one word for the same object or concept. Current research leads to the belief that this can add to the cognitive flexibility of the child. The different connotations and ideas around a word has in the different languages the child knows allows the child to build a more complex understanding of the word at a younger age.
In the book "In Other Words"[1] by Ellen Bialystok and Kenji Hakuta the authors talk about the idea that "the knowledge of two languages is greater than the sum of its parts." Basically the idea they are striving towards is that the benefits from being bilingual go much further than simply knowing two languages. Because the structures and ideas of the two languages are so different it forces the child to think in more complicated ways than if they were learning only one language.
One of the benefits of language acquisition is the increase of metalinguistic awareness, that is, a greater sensitivity to language in general and a greater awareness of meaning and structure in language.[citation needed] One reason this is speculated to be true is that multilingual children receive more linguistic input, requiring a greater amount of language analysis on the part of the child.
Cummins suggested that if L1 (first language learned) has not reached a certain threshold of competence, then the child may become “semi-lingual”, which reflects low levels of competence in both languages.[2] This theory proposes that "negative cognitive and academic effects. . . result from low levels of competence in both languages". While bilingualism has generally been considered to be of cognitive benefit, some studies have shown that it has negative effects on cognitive and academic progress. While this controversy is ongoing, Cummins explained the negative results of these studies as being associated with linguistic minorities, where the minority language was being replaced in some sense by the socially dominant one, while the studies that found a positive effect were associated with "additive bilingualism," a situation in which majority-language children acquire a second language.
[edit] Inhibitory control
A study done by Ellen Bialystok and Michelle Martin[3] showed that bilingual children have better inhibitory control for ignoring perceptual information. The theory is that children who are bilingual are constantly sorting out extra perceptual information. For every object and action they have two words, one in each language, they could use. However, they need to choose which one to use based on the context they are in and the rules that apply to that context. This ability enhances the ability of bilinguals to selectively pay attention to appropriate information and inhibit focusing on other information. Through a card sort task, this research showed that this does seem to be the case.
This ability lends itself to application in other areas of education also. For example, in mathematical problem solving, many times the first part of the problem is understanding what the question is asking and figuring out what information helps in answering that question and what other information must be found.
[edit] Impact on literacy
A study done by Ellen Bialystok, Gigi Luk and Ernest Kwan[4] showed the impact of knowing one language and writing system on learning another. They compared a group of monolinguals and three groups of bilinguals, with different relationships between English and the second language: for Spanish-English bilinguals the languages are similar and they are both written alphabetically in the same script, for Hebrew-English bilinguals the languages are different but they are both written alphabetically (phonetic Hebrew) in different scripts, for Chinese-English bilinguals both the language and the writing system are different.
The results showed that Spanish-English and Hebrew-English biliterates had the highest levels of literacy. Their interpretation of the results is that bilingualism has two effects on early acquisition of literacy: (1) a general understanding of reading and its basis in a print system and (2) the potential for transfer of reading principles across languages. All bilinguals showed an advantage in these areas over monolinguals, but the more similar the two languages the larger the advantage.
[edit] See also
[edit] References
- ^ Hakuta, Kenji; Bialystok, Ellen (1994). In other words: the science and psychology of second-language acquisition. New York: BasicBooks. ISBN 0465075657.
- ^ Cummins, J. (1979) Cognitive/academic language proficiency, linguistic interdependence, the optimum age question and some other matters. Working Papers on Bilingualism, No. 19, 121-129.
- ^ Bialystok E, Martin MM (2004). "Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task". Dev Sci 7 (3): 325–39. doi: . PMID 15595373.
- ^ Bialystok E, Luk G, Kwan E (2005). "Bilingualism, Biliteracy, and Learning to Read: Interactions Among Languages and Writing Systems". Scientific Studies of Reading 9 (1): 43–61. doi: .