Absolute threshold of hearing
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
The absolute threshold of hearing (ATH) is the minimum sound level of a pure tone that an average ear with normal hearing can hear in a noiseless environment. The absolute threshold relates to the sound that can just be heard by the organism (Durrant & Lovrinic 1984, Gelfand 2004). The absolute threshold is not a discrete point, and is therefore classed as the point at which a response is elicited a specified percentage of the time (Durrant & Lovrinic 1984). The threshold of hearing is generally reported as the sound pressure level (SPL) of 20 µPa (micropascals) = 2 × 10−5 Pascal (Pa). This is equivalent to 2 × 10−4 dynes per square centimeter. It is approximately the minimum sound intensity a young human with undamaged hearing can detect at 1000 Hz (Gelfand, 1990). The threshold of hearing is frequency dependent and it has been shown that the ear’s sensitivity is best at frequencies between 1 kHz and 5 kHz (Gelfand, 1990).
[edit] Psychophysical Methods for Measuring Thresholds
Measurement of the absolute hearing threshold provides some basic information about our auditory system (Gelfand, 1990). The tools used to collect such information are called psychophysical methods. Through these, the perception of a physical stimulus (sound) and our psychological response to the sound is measured (Hirsh, 1952).
There are several different psychophysical methods which can be used for the measurement of absolute threshold. These methods may vary in many ways; however, certain aspects are identical. Firstly, the stimulus is defined, and the manner by which the person should respond is clearly specified. The sound is then presented to the listener and the level of the stimulus is manipulated in a predetermined pattern. The absolute threshold is defined statistically, often as an average of all obtained hearing thresholds (Gelfand, 1990).
Some procedures use a series of trials, with each trial using the ‘Single-interval “yes”/”no” paradigm’. This means that sound may be present or absent in the single interval, and the listener has to say whether he thought the stimulus was there. When the interval does not contain a stimulus, it is called a "catch trial" (Gelfand, 1990).
[edit] Classical Methods
Classical methods date back to the 19th century and were first described by Gustav Theodor Fechner in his work ‘Elements of Psychophysics’ (Hirsh, 1952). Three methods are traditionally used for testing a subject’s perception of a stimulus: the method of limits, the method of constant stimuli, and the method of adjustment (Gelfand, 1990).
- Method of limits
In the method of limits, the tester controls the level of the stimuli. Single-interval “yes”/”no” paradigm’ is used, but there are no catch trials. There are several series of descending and ascending runs. The trial starts with the descending run, where a stimulus is presented at a level well above the expected threshold. When the subject responds correctly to the stimulus, the level of intensity of the sound is decreased by a specific amount and presented again. The same pattern is repeated until the subject stops responding to the stimuli, at which point the descending run is finished. In the ascending run which comes after, the stimulus is first presented well below the threshold and then gradually increased in two decibel (dB) steps until the subject responds.
As there are no clear margins to ‘hearing’ and ‘not hearing’, the threshold for each run is determined as the midpoint between the last audible and first inaudible level. The subject’s absolute hearing threshold is calculated as the mean of all obtained thresholds in both ascending and descending runs.
There are several issues related to the method of limits. First is anticipation, which is caused by the subject’s awareness that the turn-points determine a change in response. Anticipation produces better ascending thresholds and worse descending thresholds. Habituation creates completely opposite effect, and occurs when the subject becomes accustomed to responding either “yes” in the descending runs and/or “no” in the ascending runs. For this reason, thresholds are raised in ascending runs and improved in descending runs. Another problem may be related to step size. Too large a step compromises accuracy of the measurement as the actual threshold may be just between two stimulus levels. Finally, since the tone is always present, “yes” is always the correct answer (Gelfand, 1990).
- Method of Constant Stimuli
In the method of constant stimuli, the tester sets the level of stimuli and presents them at completely random order. Thus, there are no ascending or descending trials. The subject responds “yes”/”no” after each presentation. The stimuli are presented many times at each level and the threshold is defined as the stimulus level at which the subject scored 50% correct. “Catch” trials may be included in this method.
Method of constant stimuli has several advantages over the method of limits. Firstly, the random order of stimuli means that the correct answer cannot be predicted by the listener. Secondarily, as the tone may be absent (catch trial), “yes” is not always the correct answer. Finally, catch trials help to detect the amount of a listener’s guessing. The main disadvantage lies in the large number of trials which are needed to obtain the data and therefore long time required to complete the testing (Gelfand, 1990).
- Method of Adjustment
Method of adjustment shares some features with the method of limits, but differs in others. There are descending and ascending runs and the listener knows that the stimulus is always presents.
However, unlike in the method of limits, here the stimulus is controlled by the listener. The subject reduces the level of the tone until it cannot be detected anymore, or increases until it can be heard again. The stimulus level is varied continuously via a dial and the stimulus level is measured by the tester at the end. The threshold is the mean of the just audible and just inaudible levels.
Also this method can produce several biases. In order to avoid giving cues about the actual stimulus level, the dial must be unlabeled. Apart from already mentioned anticipation and habituation, stimulus persistence (preservation) could influence the result from the method of adjustment. In the descending runs, the subject may continue to reduce the level of the sound as if the sound was still audible, even though the stimulus is already well below the actual hearing threshold. In contrast, in the ascending runs, the subject may have persistence of the absence of the stimulus until the hearing threshold is passed by certain amount (Hirsh & Watson, 1996).
[edit] Modified Classical Methods
[edit] Forced-choice Methods
Two intervals are presented to a listener, one with a tone and one without a tone. Listener must decide which interval had the tone in it. The number of the intervals can be increased, but this may cause problems to the listener who has to remember which interval contained the tone (Gelfand, 1990, Miller et al, 2002).
[edit] Adaptive Methods
Unlike the classical methods, where the pattern for changing the stimuli is preset, in adaptive methods the subject’s response to the previous stimuli determines the level at which a subsequent stimulus is presented (Levitt,1971).
- 'Staircase’ Methods (Up-down methods)
The simple ‘1-down-1-up’ method consists of series of descending and ascending trials runs and turning points (reversals). The stimulus level is increased if the subject does not respond and decreased when a response occurs.
Similarly, as in the method of limits, the stimuli are adjusted in predetermined steps. After obtaining from six to eight reversals, the first one is discarded and the threshold is defined as the average of the midpoints of the remaining runs. Experiments showed that this method provides only 50% accuracy (Levitt, 1971). In order to produce more accurate results, this simple method can be further modified by increasing the size of steps in the descending runs, e.g. ‘2-down-1-up method’, ‘3-down-1-up methods’ (Gelfand, 1990).
- Bekesy’s Tracking Method
Bekesy’s method contains some aspects of classical methods and staircase methods. The level of the stimulus is automatically varied at a fixed rate. The subject is asked to press a button when the stimulus is detectable.
Once the button is pressed, the level is automatically decreased by the motor-driven attenuator and increased when the button is not pushed. The threshold is thus tracked by the listeners, and calculated as the mean of the midpoints of the runs as recorded by the automat (Gelfand, 1990).
[edit] Hysteresis Effect
Hysteresis can be defined as ‘the lagging of an effect behind its cause’ http://www.thefreedictionary.com/hysteresis
When measuring hearing thresholds it is always easier for the subject to follow a tone that is audible and decreasing in amplitude than to detect a tone that was previously inaudible. This is because ‘top-down’ influences mean that the subject will be expecting to hear the sound and will, therefore, be more motivated with higher levels of concentration. The ‘bottom-up’ theory explains that unwanted external (from the environment) and internal (e.g. heartbeat) noise will result in the subject only responding to the sound if the signal to noise ratio is above a certain amount.
In practice this means that when measuring threshold with sounds decreasing in amplitude, the point at which the sound becomes inaudible will always be lower than the point at which it returns to audibility. This phenomenon is known as the ‘hysteresis effect’.
[edit] Psychometric Function of Absolute Hearing Threshold
Psychometric function ‘represents the probability of a certain listener’s response as a function of the magnitude of the particular sound characteristic being studied’ (Arlinger, 1991).
To give an example, this could be the probability curve of the subject detecting a sound being presented as a function of the sound level. When the stimulus is presented to the listener one would expect that the sound would either be audible or inaudible, resulting in a 'doorstep' function. In reality a grey area exists where the listener is uncertain as to whether they have actually heard the sound or not, so their responses are inconsistent, resulting in a psychometric function.
The psychometric function is a sigmoid function which is characterised by being ‘s’ shaped in its graphical representation.
[edit] Minimal Audible Field (MAF) vs Minimal Audible Pressure (MAP)
Two methods can be used to measure the minimal audible stimulus (Gelfand 2004) and therefore the absolute threshold of hearing. Minimal audible field involves the subject sitting in a sound field and stimulus being presented via a loudspeaker (Gelfand 2004, Kidd 2002). The sound level is then measured at the position of the subjects head with the subject not in the sound field (Gelfand 2004). Minimal audible pressure involves presenting stimuli via headphones (Gelfand 2004) or earphones (Durrant & Lovrinic 1984, Kidd 2002) and measuring sound pressure in the subject’s ear canal using a very small probe microphone (Gelfand 2004). The two different methods produce different thresholds (Durrant & Lovrinic 1984, Gelfand 2004) and minimal audible field thresholds are often 6-10dB better than minimal audible pressure thresholds (Gelfand 2004). It is thought that this difference is due to:
- monaural vs binaural hearing. With minimal audible field both ears are able to detect the stimuli but with minimal audible pressure only one ear is able to detect the stimuli. Binaural hearing is more sensitive than monaural hearing (Durrant & Lovrinic 1984).
- physiological noises heard when ear is occluded by an earphone during minimal audible pressure measurements (Gelfand 2004). When the ear is covered the subject will hear body noises, such as heart beat, and these may have a masking effect.
Minimal audible field and minimal audible pressure are important when considering calibration issues and they also illustrate that the human hearing is most sensitive in the 2-5kHz range (Gelfand 2004).
[edit] Reference Equivalent Sound Pressure Level (RETSPL)
Standardisation of audiometric equipment must be carried out, so that hearing loss relative to “normal hearing” can be quantified. This is based on the SPL needed for the average person to detect a sound. Reference equivalent sound pressure levels (RETSPLs) are standards we use for normal hearing. They are reference values for pure tone signals presented from various kinds of earphones. The values are based upon a round robin of loudness-balance and threshold experiments involving American, British, French, Russian, and German test centres. The reference levels obtained here have been incorporated into the American National Standards Institute. Because the RETSPL at each frequency represents the populations’ average hearing threshold we may think of them as all representing the same hearing level. Thus each RETSPL may also be referred to as 0-dB Hearing level (0-dBHL). For example a reference level for a 1000Hz tone may be 7, so that 0dBHL corresponds to 7.5 dB SPL at 1000Hz. At 250Hz, more sound pressure is required for the average listener to reach the normal threshold so 0 dBHL for this frequency is 26.5 dB SPL. When measuring bone conduction thresholds we use reference-equivalent threshold force levels (RETFLs) because they express the equivalent force on a measuring device called an artificial mastoid, which corresponds to 0 dB HL when the bone-conduction vibrator is placed upon a person’s mastoid (Gelfand, 2004).
[edit] Temporal Summation
Temporal summation is the relationship between stimulus duration and intensity when the presentation time is less than 1 second. Auditory sensitivity changes when the duration of a sound becomes less than 1 second. The threshold intensity decreases by about 10 dB when the duration of a tone burst is increased from 20 - 200 ms.
For example the most quiet sound a subject can hear is 16 dB if the sound is presented at a duration of 200 ms. If the same sound at 16 dB is then presented for a duration of 20 ms only the most quiet sound that can now be heard by the subject goes up to 26dB. In other words if a signal is shortened by a factor of 10 then the level of that signal must be increased by as much as 10 dB to be heard by the subject.
The ear operates as an energy detector that samples the amount of energy present within a certain time frame. A certain amount of energy is needed within a time frame to reach the threshold. This can be done by using a higher intensity for less time or by using a lower intensity for more time. Sensitivity to sound improves as the signal duration increases up to about 200-300 ms, after that the threshold remains constant (Gelfand 2004).
[edit] Frequency Variation
The audible frequency range is usually quoted as 20 Hz to 20 000 Hz. Obtaining thresholds is reliant on stimuli being presented in frequencies within the Auditory Response Area.
[edit] See Also
- DB(A)
- Equal-loudness contour
- Loudness
- Phon
- Psychoacoustics
- Psychophysics
- Signal detection theory
- Sone
[edit] References
Arlinger, S. 1991. Manual of Practical Audiometry: Volume 2 (Practical Aspects of Audiology). Chichester: Whurr Publishers.
Durrant J D., Lovrinic J H. 1984. Bases of Hearing Sciences. Second Edition. United States of America: Williams & Wilkins
Fechner, G., 1860. Elements of psychophysics. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Citation from the book available on: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Fechner/.
Gelfand, S A., 1990. Hearing: An introduction to psychological and physiological acoustics. 2nd edition. New York and Basel: Marcel Dekker, Inc.
Gelfand S A., 2004. Hearing an Introduction to Psychological and Physiological Acoustics. Fourth edition. United States of America: Marcel Dekker
Hirsh I J.,1952. "The Measurement of Hearing". United States of America: McGraw-Hill.
Hirsh I J.,Watson C S., 1996. Auditory Psychophysics and Perception. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 47: 461-84. Available to download from: arjournals.annualreviews.org/doi/pdf/10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.461 . [Accessed 01 March 2007].
Kidd G. 2002. Psychoacoustics IN Handbook of Clinical Audiology. Fifth Edition.
Katz J. (Ed). United States of America: Lippencott, Williams & Wilkins
Levitt H., 1971. "Trasformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics". J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 49, 467-477. Available to download from: http://scitation.aip.org/getabs/servlet/GetabsServlet?prog=normal&id=JASMAN00004900002B000467000001&idtype=cvips&gifs=yes. [Accessed 01 March 2007}.
Miller et al, 2002. "Nonparametric relationships between single-interval and two-interval forced-choice tasks in the theory of signal detectability". Journal of Mathematical Psychology archive. 46:4; 383 - 417. Available from: http://portal.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=634580. [Accessed 01 March 2007].
www.thefreedictionary.com. [Accessed 28 February 2007]
[edit] External links
A comparison of threshold estimation methods in children 6–11 years of age
A Concise Vocabulary of Audiology and allied topics
Fundamental aspects of hearing
Equal loudness contours and audiometry - Test your own hearing
Fundamentals of psychoacoustics
Minimising boredom by maximising likelihood-an efficient estimation of masked thresholds
On Minimum Audible Sound Fields
Psychometric Functions for Children's Detection of Tones in Noise
Reference levels for objective audiometry
Response bias in psychophysics
The psychoacoustics of multichannel audio
Three Models of Temporal Summation Evaluated Using Normal-Hearing and Hearing-Impaired Subjects
Threshold this is coolio