Dyslexia
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
ICD-10 | F81.0, R48.0 |
---|---|
ICD-9 | 315.02, 784.61 |
OMIM | 127700 127700 604254 606896 606616 608995 300509 |
DiseasesDB | 4016 |
MeSH | D004410 |
Developmental dyslexia is a condition or learning disability which causes difficulty with reading and writing.
Its standard definition is a difficulty in reading and writing in spite of normal development of intelligence, cognitive and sensory abilities.
The word "dyslexia" comes from the Greek words δυς- dys- ("impaired") and λέξις lexis ("words" or "lexicon"). People are often identified as dyslexic when their reading or writing problems cannot be explained by a lack of intellectual ability, inadequate instruction, or sensory problems such as poor eyesight.
The term dyslexia is also sometimes used to refer to the loss of reading ability following brain damage. This form of dyslexia is more often referred to as either acquired dyslexia or "Alexia". Dyslexia primarily impacts reading and writing abilities; however, other difficulties have been reported including deficits in processing spoken language[1] as well as non-language difficulties[2].
Dyslexia is not limited to reversing the order of letters in reading or writing. Nor is it a visual perception deficit that involves reading letters or words backwards or upside down, as is often implied in popular culture.
Researchers have claimed that it is a brain-based condition with biochemical and genetic markers[3][4][5]. Others have questioned whether dyslexia is no more than a mythological construct and argue that researchers that rely on the concept fail to recognize neurodiversity. Its diagnostic status remains highly debated in both medicine and the social sciences [6].
Contents |
[edit] History
The term 'dyslexia' was coined in 1887 by Rudolf Berlin [7]. He used the term to refer to a case of a young boy who had a severe impairment in learning to read and write in spite of showing typical intellectual and physical abilities in all other respects.
Several years later, W. Pringle Morgan, a British physician, published a description of a reading-specific learning disorder in a letter to the British Medical Journal. His letter described the case of a boy named Percy who, at age 14, had not yet learned to read, yet he showed normal intelligence and was generally adept at other activities typical of children of that age.
Much of the scientific research focusing on dyslexia throughout the 20th century focused on the idea that dyslexia stems from a visual deficit. This notion has persisted in popular culture, where it is commonly believed that dyslexia involves reading words backwards or upside-down. Nevertheless, research has failed to demonstrate that individuals with dyslexia perceive words or other visual objects differently from individuals with typical reading abilities.
A key early researcher in dyslexia was Samuel T. Orton. Orton coined the term strephosymbolia (meaning 'twisted signs') to describe his theory that individuals with dyslexia had difficulty associating the visual forms of words with their spoken forms [8]. Orton observed that reading deficits in dyslexia did not seem to stem from strictly visual deficits[9]. He also noted that many individuals with dyslexia were also left-handed, although this finding has been difficult to replicate and its significance continues to be hotly debated[10].
In the 1970s, a new hypothesis emerged that suggested dyslexia stems from a deficit in processing the phonological form of speech. This theory suggests that reading problems in dyslexia stem from difficulty decomposing spoken words into discrete phonemes (for example, breaking the word CAT into [k], [æ], and [t] sounds). As a result, affected individuals have difficulty associating these sounds with the visual letters that make up written words. Key studies of the phonological deficit hypothesis include the finding that the strongest predictor of reading success in school age children is phonological awareness[11], and that phonological awareness instruction can improve reading scores in children with reading difficulties[12].
The Design versus Deficit debate — Thomas G. West, towards the end of the 20th century, suggested the theory that dyslexia may be design not deficit related, citing Galaburda and his own research. West suggests that many dyslexics belong to a much larger group of visual spatial thinkers who are wired for the big picture — designed to process information visually. In is only in a secondary state that dyslexics come to process information in a logical, sequential, language-based context. The uncomfortable reality may be that our education system indirectly attempts to screen out the Einstein gene, and thereby all our most original and gifted thinkers. West goes on to examine the difficult early experiences within education of five Nobel prize winners. or near-winners: Einstein, Edison, Marconi, Churchill and Faraday.
West's theory is echoed in the work of Ronald Dell Davis, author of The Gift of Dyslexia, who describes dyslexia as the outgrowth of a primarily picture-thinking mind. Davis posits that the symptoms associated with dyslexia arise from disorientation that results from confusion over language symbols. This view has also been supported indirectly by the research of by Linda Silverman, author of Upside Down Brilliance - the title reflects the counter-intuitive experiences of those who find the easy tasks difficult and hard tasks easy.
Echoes of this theory can be traced to the emerging discipline of NLP, the origin of VAK , now backed in the UK by the DFES [Department for Education and Skills ]. Robin Williams of ABC, extended the theory to explain the paradox of bright under-achievers in his 2000 broadcast, The Einstein factor. The importance of a design-based theory is that design-based solutions are quite different from deficit solutions. A design solution suggests that literacy as a focus of the condition dyslexia must be considered a symptom and not the condition, that Professor Joe Elliott, the man most closely associated with 'The Dyslexia Myth documentary, may be right but for the wrong reasons. That IQ, as Elliott has argued, has nothing to do with literacy. This is where Elliott stops. A design theory argues that dyslexia as a condition is the result of a trade off and by identifying that trade off you can use strengths to offset the constellation of traits which constitute dyslexia , a condition related to a single root cause a difficulty with language [ dys - difficulty & lexia — language ] in all its forms: writing, reading, oral presentations and importantly memory. In other words, design not deficit.
[edit] Dyslexia definitions
Dyslexia is widely accepted to be a specific learning disability. That is, dyslexia has biological traits that differentiate it from other learning disabilities. However, the specific definition of dyslexia varies somewhat across communities.
Dyslexia or Reading Disorder is defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV as reading achievement that falls substantially below expected levels given an individual's age and education. The reading deficit should be sufficiently severe as to interfere with everyday activities requiring reading (e.g., schoolwork or employment). Finally, the reading deficit cannot be strictly due to a sensory disorder; for instance, it cannot be strictly due to vision problems that prevent an individual from seeing words on a page.
Several national and international organizations have also set out definitions of dyslexia as follows:
The World Health Organization (WHO)
"A disorder manifested by difficulty learning to read, despite conventional instruction, adequate intelligence and sociocultural opportunity. It is dependent upon fundamental cognitive disabilities which are frequently of constitutional origin". ICD-10, The International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, tenth revision ICIDH-2, The International Classification of Impairments, Activities, and Participation
US National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) / International Dyslexia Association
Defines Dyslexia as a specific learning disability of neurological origin. Characterized with difficulties with accurate and/or fluent word recognition, spelling and decoding abilities.
Canadian Government
The Government of Canada’s Health Portal links its description to the BC HealthGuide web site using their definition. Dyslexia is defined here having a difficulty with the alphabet, reading, writing, and spelling in spite of normal to above average intelligence, conventional teaching, and adequate sociocultural opportunity. Dyslexia is thought to be both genetic and hereditary. Dyslexia is not caused by poor vision. Dyslexia is identified following psychological and educational tests that determine language and other academic abilities, IQ and problem-solving skills, and is only identified if the reading disability is not a result of another condition.
The British Dyslexia Association
Dyslexia is a difference in the brain area that deals with language. It affects the underlying skills that are needed for learning to read, write and spell. Brain imaging techniques show that dyslexic people process information differently. In a report on the House of Lords Dyslexia debate which took place on December 7, 2005, the government confirms that dyslexia is not a myth.
[edit] Biological bases of dyslexia
Developmental dyslexia appears to be the result of differences in affected individuals' neural organization for language and reading. Developmental dyslexia also appears to have a genetic component, such that it can tend to occur in multiple members of the same family. Reading difficulties in dyslexia can vary in their severity. The condition is not restricted to childhood, but can persist through adulthood. In addition, while early reports suggested dyslexia is more prevalent in boys, more recent studies have indicated it is not sex-linked, and occurs both in boys and girls with equal frequency.
Most theories focus on non-primary areas in the frontal lobe and the temporal lobe[13]. Studies have linked several forms of dyslexia to genetic markers[14][15][16].
[edit] Variations and related conditions
Dyslexia is a learning disorder. Its underlying cause is believed to be a brain-based condition that influences the ability to read written language. It is identified in individuals who fail to learn to read in the absence of a verbal or nonverbal intellectual impairment, sensory deficit (e.g., a visual deficit or hearing loss), pervasive developmental deficit or a frank neurological impairment. The following conditions are sometimes confused with dyslexia because they can also lead to difficulty reading:
- Auditory Processing Disorder is a condition that affects the ability to encode auditory information. It can lead to problems with auditory working memory and auditory sequencing.
- Dyspraxia is a neurological condition characterized by a marked difficulty in carrying out routine tasks involving balance, fine-motor control, and kinesthetic coordination. This is most common in dyslexics who also have an attention deficit disorder.
- Verbal Dyspraxia is a neurological condition characterized by marked difficulty in the use of speech sounds, which is the result of an immaturity in the speech production area of the brain.
- Dysgraphia is a neurological condition characterized by distorted and incorrect handwriting.
- Dyscalculia is a neurological condition characterized by a problem with learning fundamentals and one or more of the basic numerical skills. Often people with this condition can understand very complex mathematical concepts and principles but have difficulty processing formulas and even basic addition and subtraction.
- Scotopic Sensitivity also known as Irlen Syndrome is a sensitivity to certain wavelengths of light which interfere with proper visual processing.
[edit] Facts and statistics
Dyslexia's main manifestation is a difficulty in developing reading skills in elementary school children. Those difficulties result from reduced ability to associate visual symbols with verbal sounds. While motivational factors must also be reviewed in assessing poor performance, dyslexia is considered to be present from birth. Most scientific criteria for dyslexia exclude cases that can be explained as arising from environmental factors such as lack of education or sensory deficits.
Much of the current scientific research focuses on the hypothesis that dyslexia stems from a deficit in phonological awareness. This hypothesis suggests that affected individuals have difficulty analyzing the words they hear into discrete segments (such as phonemes), which in turn leads to difficulty learning spelling-sound correspondences.
Other research focuses on the theory that dyslexia results from a magnocellular deficit related to visual processing[1], or that it is related to a cerebellar deficit. [2]
Dyslexia can be substantially compensated for with proper therapy, training, and equipment.
The current consensus is that dyslexia occurs in both sexes with equal frequency. It was previously reported more frequently in males, possibly due to selection factors and/or bias.
Although they are different conditions, dyslexia co-occurs with attention deficit disorders (ADD or ADHD) at a rate of 30-50%.
[edit] Treatment
The most common form of treatment for dyslexia in English-speaking countries is through educational tutoring, often using an approach based on Orton-Gillingham, which provides systematic, multisensory teaching geared to building phonetic decoding skills.
Remedial efforts focusing on phonological awareness training (often involving breaking words into their basic sounds and rearranging these sounds to produce different words) can improve reading decoding skills. The earlier the phonological regimen is taken on, the better the overall result.
Other popular educationally-based methods include Lindamood-Bell Learning Processes, which provides extensive tutoring geared to developing precursor and ancillary reading skills, such as Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing (LiPS) Program, to help develop phonemic awareness [3]; and Phono-Graphix, which is an alternative multisensory approach to teaching phonetic skills, designed to be faster paced and to avoid the repetition and drill associated with traditional tutoring methods.[4]
There are also a number of therapeutic approaches which focus on providing treatment for underlying cognitive, neurological, auditory, or visual impairments thought to underlie dyslexia. These include the following: Davis Dyslexia Correction, which is a comprehensive, counseling-based approach combining mental techniques to overcome disorientation associated with dyslexia with the use of clay modeling to learn the alphabet, words and concepts; Fast ForWord, a software training program geared to improving children's ability to distinguish the sounds of language; the Dore Program (DDAT), a system of balance and coordination exercises designed to increase cerebellar function; Auditory integration training; and Neurofeedback.
There is also evidence that colored lenses, vision therapy, or similar proposed treatments may be of use in overcoming reading problems.
[edit] Physiology
Researchers studying the brains of dyslexics have found that during reading tasks, dyslexics show reduced activity in the left inferior parietal cortex. It is anecdotally claimed that it is not that uncommon for dyslexics who have trained themselves to cope with their affliction to develop uncannily efficient visual memories which aid in reading and comprehending large quantities of information much faster than is typical. Some dyslexics may show a natural dislike of reading and, as a consequence, compensate by developing unique verbal communication skills, interpersonal expertise, visual-spatial abilities, and leadership skills.
In 1979, anatomical differences in the brain of a young dyslexic were documented. Albert Galaburda of Harvard Medical School noticed that the language center in a dyslexic brain showed microscopic flaws known as ectopias and microgyria. Both affect the normal six-layer structure of the cortex. An ectopia is a collection of neurons that have pushed up from the lower layers of the cortex into the outermost one. A microgyrus is an area of cortex that includes only four layers instead of six. These flaws affect connectivity and functionality of the cortex in critical areas related to auditory processing and visual processing, which seems consistent with the hypothesis that dyslexia stems from a phonological awareness deficit.
Another study regarding genetic regions on chromosomes 1 and 6 have been found that might be linked to dyslexia. Presenting the argument, dyslexia is a conglomeration of conditions that all affect similar and associated areas of the cortex.
[edit] Effect of language orthography
Some studies have concluded that speakers of languages whose orthography has a strong correspondence between letter and sound (e.g. Korean, Italian and Spanish) suffer less from effects of dyslexia than speakers of languages where the letter is less closely linked to the sound (e.g. English and French).[17]
In one of these studies, reported in Seymour et al.,[18] the word-reading accuracy of first-grade children of different European languages was measured. English children had an accuracy of just 40%, whereas among children of most other European languages accuracy was about 95%, with French and Danish children somewhere in the middle at about 75%; Danish and French are known to have an irregular pronunciation.
However, this does not mean that dyslexia is caused by orthography: instead, Ziegler et al.[19] claim that the dyslexia suffered by German or Italian dyslectics is of the same kind as the one suffered by the English ones, supporting the theory that the origin of dyslexia is biological. However, dyslexia has more pronounced effects on more "difficult" languages.
[edit] Characteristics
Diagnosis of dyslexia is made by a qualified medical professional. It is typically made on an exclusionary basis—that is, dyslexia is only identified when reading difficulties cannot be explained as resulting from a more general impairment affecting sensory or neurological abilities.
Recent advances in neuroimaging and genetics could help identify children at risk of dyslexia before they learn to read, however none of these techniques are currently being used for this purpose. Currently, the only way to positively diagnose dyslexia is through behavioral testing.
The following characteristics have been adapted from R. D. Davis, 37 Common Characteristics of Dyslexia.[20]
[edit] General
Individuals with dyslexia:
- Appear bright, intelligent, and articulate but are unable to read, write, or spell at an age-appropriate level.
- Have average or above average intelligence, yet may have poor academic achievement; may have good oral language abilities but will perform much more poorly on similar written-language tests.
- Might be labelled lazy, dumb, careless, immature, "not trying hard enough," or as having a "behavior problem."
- Because dyslexia primarily affects reading while sparing other intellectual abilities, affected individuals might be categorised as not "behind enough" or "bad enough" to receive additional help in a school setting.
- Might feel dumb and have poor self-esteem, and might be easily frustrated and emotional about school reading or testing.
- Might try to hide their reading weaknesses with ingenious compensatory "strategies".
- Might learn best through hands-on experience, demonstrations, experimentation, observation, and visual aids.
- Can show talents in other areas such as art, drama, music, sports, mechanics, story-telling, sales, business, designing, building, or engineering.
- Have related problems with inattention in a school setting; for instance they might seem to "zone out" or daydream often; get lost easily or lose track of time; and have difficulty sustaining attention.
[edit] Vision, reading, and spelling
Contrary to how it has been portrayed in the popular press, individuals with dyslexia do not perceive words backwards or upside down. In fact, visual problems are typically ruled out before a diagnosis of dyslexia can be made. Early studies of dyslexia did focus on the possibility that dyslexia is caused by visual difficulties, however very little evidence was found to support this theory. Likewise, there is little evidence that visual training provides effective treatment.[21].
- A popular theory in the 1970s suggested dyslexics may have Meares-Irlen Syndrome, a visual deficit in which the glare of the white page against black letters causes the words to shake, shiver, spin, etc. after a few minutes of reading. The proposed treatment was the use of colored plastic overlays that were thought help anchor the words to the page. This treatment has fallen out of favor as scientific studies have failed to support these claims. (See link below to asfedia).
- Spelling errors - Because of difficulty learning letter-sound correspondences, individuals with dyslexia might tend to misspell words, or leave vowels out of words (e.g., spelling "magic" as mjc).
- Reading - Due to dyslexics' excellent long term memory, young students tend to memorize beginning readers, but are unable to read individual words or phrases.
[edit] Writing and motor skills
Because of literacy problems, an individual with dyslexia may have difficulty with handwriting. This can involve slower writing speed than average or poor handwriting characterised by irregular formed letters.
Some studies have also reported gross motor difficulties in dyslexia, including motor skills disorder. This difficulty is indicated by clumsiness and poor coordination. The relationship between motor skills and reading difficulties is poorly understood but could be linked to the role of the cerebellum in the development of reading and motor abilities.[22].
[edit] Math abilities
Dyslexia should not be confused with dyscalculia, a learning disability marked by severe difficulties with mathematics. Individuals with dyslexia can be gifted in math while having poor reading skills. However, in spite of this they might have difficulty with word problems (i.e., math problems that rely on written text rather than numbers or formulas). It is also possible that individuals with dyslexia have difficulty with multiplication tables, and other mathematics which involve remembering the order in which numbers appear.
[edit] Public support
In the United States, Canada, New Zealand and in the United Kingdom, some people say that there is a lack of adequate support and a general lack of interest in the learning disabilities of children in public schools. This has recently led to legal action by private parties against public schools in the United States and state schools in the United Kingdom. In English law, the recent case of Skipper v Calderdale Metropolitan Borough School (2006) EWCA Civ 238 the Court of Appeal applied Phelps v London Borough of Hillingdon (2001) 2 AC 619 as the landmark case on the failure to diagnose dyslexia (see duty of care in English law), and to hold that the appellant could pursue her claim against her school for humiliation, lost confidence, and lost self-esteem, and for loss of earnings following its failing to diagnose and treat her dyslexia despite the fact that, as Latham LJ. says at para 29:
- "The extent to which her dyslexia could have been ameliorated or provided for will always remain uncertain, as will the extent to which that would have affected her performance in public examinations; the evidence that we have includes material to suggest that she, not surprisingly, reacted adversely to the break-up of her parents marriage when she was 15, in other words at a critical time in her education. Whether any improvement in her examination results would have led to her life taking a significantly different course will also be a matter for some speculation."
Some charitable organizations like the Scottish Rite Foundation have undertaken the task of testing for dyslexia and making training classes and materials available, often without cost, for teachers and students.[23][24][25]
In England and Wales, the failure of schools to diagnose and provide remedial help for dyslexia following the House of Lords decision in the case of Pamela Phelps has created an entitlement for students with dyslexia in Higher Education to receive support funded via the Disabled Students Allowance. Support can take the form of IT equipment (software and hardware) as well as personal assistance, also known as non-medical helper support. Dyslexic students will also be entitled to special provision in examinations such as additional time to allow them to read and comprehend exam questions.
The British Disability Discrimination Act also covers dyslexia.
- "In some cases, people have 'coping strategies' which cease to work in certain circumstances (for example, where someone who stutters or has dyslexia is placed under stress). If it is possible that a person's ability to manage the effects of the impairment will break down so that these effects will sometimes occur, this possibility must be taken into account when assessing the effects of the impairment." — Paragraph A8, Guidance to the Definitions of Disability
[edit] Controversy
Some disagreement exists as to whether dyslexia does indeed exist as a condition, or whether it simply reflects individual differences among different readers.
"The Dyslexia Myth" is a documentary that appeared as part of the Dispatches series produced by British broadcaster Channel 4.[26]. First aired in September 2005, it claims to expose myths and misconceptions that surround dyslexia. It argues that the common understanding of dyslexia is not only false but makes it more difficult to provide the reading help that hundreds of thousands of children desperately need. Drawing on years of intensive academic research on both sides of the Atlantic, it challenged the existence of dyslexia as a separate condition, and highlighted the many different forms of reading style.
[edit] References
- ^ Stanovich, KE. (1988) Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21(10):590-604.
- ^ Murphy, Martin F (2004). Dyslexia, An Explanation. Flyleaf Press. ISBN 0-9539974-5-6.
- ^ Cope, N, Harold D, Hill G, Moskvina V, Stevenson J, Holmans P, Owen MJ, O'Donovan MC, Williams J (April 2005). "Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia". American Journal of Human Genetics 76 (4): 581-91. PMID 15717286.
- ^ Meng, H, Smith SD, Hager K, Held M, Liu J, Olson RK, Pennington BF, DeFries JC, Gelernter J, O'Reilly-Pol T, Somlo S, Skudlarski P, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Marchione K, Wang Y, Paramasivam M, LoTurco JJ, Page GP, Gruen JR (Nov 22 2005). "DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 (47): 17053-8. PMID 16278297.
- ^ Schumacher, J, Anthoni H, Dahdouh F, Konig IR, Hillmer AM, Kluck N, Manthey M, Plume E, Warnke A, Remschmidt H, Hulsmann J, Cichon S, Lindgren CM, Propping P, Zucchelli M, Ziegler A, Peyrard-Janvid M, Schulte-Korne G, Nothen MM, Kere J (Jan 2006). "Strong genetic evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility gene for dyslexia". American Journal of Human Genetics 78 (1): 52-62. PMID 16385449.
- ^ The Dyslexia Myth. Dispatches. Channel 4.
- ^ Berlin, Rudolf (1884). "Uber Dyslexie". Archiv fur Psychiatrie 15: 276-278.
- ^ Orton, ST (1925). "'Word-blindness' in school children.". Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 14: 582–615.
- ^ Henry, MK (1998). "Structured, sequential, multisensory teaching: The Perlow legacy". Annals of Dyslexia.
- ^ Geschwind, N (1982). "Biological associations of left-handedness". Annals of Dyslexia 33: 29-40.
- ^ Bradley, L, Bryant, PE (1983). "Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A Causal connection.". Nature 30 (2): 419-421.
- ^ Alexander, A, Anderson, H, Heilman, P, Voeller, K, Torgesen, J (1991). "Phonological awareness training and the remediation of analytic decoding deficits in a group of severe dyslexics". Annals of Dyslexia 41: 193-206.
- ^ Galaburda, AM (Apr-Jun 1994). "Developmental dyslexia and animal studies: at the interface between cognition and neurology". Cognition 50 (1-3): 133-49. PMID 8039357.
- ^ Grigorenko, EL (Jan 2001). "Developmental dyslexia: an update on genes, brains, and environments". Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 42 (1): 91-125. PMID 11205626.
- ^ Grigorenko, EL, Wood FB, Meyer MS, Hart LA, Speed WC, Shuster A, Pauls DL (Jan 1997). "Susceptibility loci for distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15". American journal of human genetics 60 (1): 27-39. PMID 8981944.
- ^ Grigorenko, EL, Wood FB, Meyer MS, Pauls DL (Feb 2000). "Chromosome 6p influences on different dyslexia-related cognitive processes: further confirmation". American journal of human genetics 66 (2): 715-23. PMID 10677331.
- ^ "Scientists Say Severity of Dyslexia Depends on Language", The Tech News Briefs, Los Angeles Times, March 16, 2001. Retrieved on 2006-06-06.
- ^ Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143 – 174.
- ^ Johannes C. Ziegler, Conrad Perry, Anna Ma-Wyatt, Diana Ladner, and Gerd Schulte-Körne, Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 86 (2003) 169 – 193
- ^ Davis, Ronald D. (1992). 37 Common Characteristics of Dyslexia. Davis Dyslexia Association International. Retrieved on June 6, 2006.
- ^ Metzger, RL, Werner DB (Jun 1984). "Use of visual training for reading disabilities: a review". Pediatrics 73 (6): 824-9. PMID 6374600.
- ^ Nicolson, R. and Fawcett, A. (Nov 1999). "Developmental dyslexia: the role of the cerebellum". Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice 5: 155-7.
- ^ Dyslexia Workshop Topics. Alabama Scottish Rite Foundation Learning Centers. Retrieved on June 6, 2006.
- ^ Scottish Rite Masonic Children's Learning Centers. Retrieved on June 6, 2006.
- ^ Scottish Rite Philanthropy. The Scottish Rite Bodies of Austin. Retrieved on June 6, 2006.
- ^ The Dyslexia Myth. Dispatches. Channel 4.
[edit] See also
- Alexia (disorder)
- Asfedia
- Double deficit (education)
- Dysorthographia
- Educational psychology
- Landmark East School
- Learning disability
- List of notable people diagnosed with dyslexia
- Reactive inhibition
- Retroactive inhibition
- The Gow School
- Visual thinking
[edit] External links
- Dyslexia Treatment Guide Using the VAK discipline when learning NLP backed by DFES
- Learning Disabilities Association of America (LDA)
- Division for Learning Disabilities (DLD) of the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC)
- International Dyslexia Association (IDA)
- Help for Parents of Dyslexic Children
- Neuron Learning Fast ForWord
- LD Support Offers answers to FAQs for learning disabled and gifted students
- The Dyslexia Guide A to Z knowledgebase and FAQs
- The Initiative for Learning Identities (ili), Adults with Dyslexia (US)
- SchwabLearning.org - A Parent's Guide to Helping Kids With Learning Difficulties (US)
- Disability Rights Advocates, sponsor of the David Boise Fellow on LD Law (US)
- Samuel T Orton and June L Orton Personal Papers and Manuscripts at the Columbia University Health Science Library collection
- 'The Mathematician Who Can't Add Up' Emma King Freeview 'Snapshot' video by the Vega Science Trust
- A Framework for Understanding Dyslexia The UK Department of Education and Skills's site on dyslexia, covering the different theories and approaches.
[edit] Regional associations and organizations
- Denver Academy A private school in Denver Colorado, USA that specializes in the education of students with a wide variety "Learning Differences" such as ADD, ADHD, Dyslexia and OCD.
- Auditory Processing Disorder (UK)
- British Dyslexia Association
- Canadian Dyslexia Association
- Davis Dyslexia Association International (DDAI)
- Danks Davis Dyslexia
- Dyslexia Council (UK)
- Fawco Site for parents of Dyslexic children, who live abroad